Social Psychology Experiments: 10 Of The Most Famous Studies
Ten of the most influential social psychology experiments explain why we sometimes do dumb or irrational things.
Ten of the most influential social psychology experiments explain why we sometimes do dumb or irrational things.
“I have been primarily interested in how and why ordinary people do unusual things, things that seem alien to their natures. Why do good people sometimes act evil? Why do smart people sometimes do dumb or irrational things?” –Philip Zimbardo
Like famous social psychologist Professor Philip Zimbardo (author of The Lucifer Effect: Understanding How Good People Turn Evil ), I’m also obsessed with why we do dumb or irrational things.
The answer quite often is because of other people — something social psychologists have comprehensively shown.
Each of the 10 brilliant social psychology experiments below tells a unique, insightful story relevant to all our lives, every day.
Click the link in each social psychology experiment to get the full description and explanation of each phenomenon.
1. Social Psychology Experiments: The Halo Effect
The halo effect is a finding from a famous social psychology experiment.
It is the idea that global evaluations about a person (e.g. she is likeable) bleed over into judgements about their specific traits (e.g. she is intelligent).
It is sometimes called the “what is beautiful is good” principle, or the “physical attractiveness stereotype”.
It is called the halo effect because a halo was often used in religious art to show that a person is good.
2. Cognitive Dissonance
Cognitive dissonance is the mental discomfort people feel when trying to hold two conflicting beliefs in their mind.
People resolve this discomfort by changing their thoughts to align with one of conflicting beliefs and rejecting the other.
The study provides a central insight into the stories we tell ourselves about why we think and behave the way we do.
3. Robbers Cave Experiment: How Group Conflicts Develop
The Robbers Cave experiment was a famous social psychology experiment on how prejudice and conflict emerged between two group of boys.
It shows how groups naturally develop their own cultures, status structures and boundaries — and then come into conflict with each other.
For example, each country has its own culture, its government, legal system and it draws boundaries to differentiate itself from neighbouring countries.
One of the reasons the became so famous is that it appeared to show how groups could be reconciled, how peace could flourish.
The key was the focus on superordinate goals, those stretching beyond the boundaries of the group itself.
4. Social Psychology Experiments: The Stanford Prison Experiment
The Stanford prison experiment was run to find out how people would react to being made a prisoner or prison guard.
The psychologist Philip Zimbardo, who led the Stanford prison experiment, thought ordinary, healthy people would come to behave cruelly, like prison guards, if they were put in that situation, even if it was against their personality.
It has since become a classic social psychology experiment, studied by generations of students and recently coming under a lot of criticism.
5. The Milgram Social Psychology Experiment
The Milgram experiment , led by the well-known psychologist Stanley Milgram in the 1960s, aimed to test people’s obedience to authority.
The results of Milgram’s social psychology experiment, sometimes known as the Milgram obedience study, continue to be both thought-provoking and controversial.
The Milgram experiment discovered people are much more obedient than you might imagine.
Fully 63 percent of the participants continued administering what appeared like electric shocks to another person while they screamed in agony, begged to stop and eventually fell silent — just because they were told to.
6. The False Consensus Effect
The false consensus effect is a famous social psychological finding that people tend to assume that others agree with them.
It could apply to opinions, values, beliefs or behaviours, but people assume others think and act in the same way as they do.
It is hard for many people to believe the false consensus effect exists because they quite naturally believe they are good ‘intuitive psychologists’, thinking it is relatively easy to predict other people’s attitudes and behaviours.
In reality, people show a number of predictable biases, such as the false consensus effect, when estimating other people’s behaviour and its causes.
7. Social Psychology Experiments: Social Identity Theory
Social identity theory helps to explain why people’s behaviour in groups is fascinating and sometimes disturbing.
People gain part of their self from the groups they belong to and that is at the heart of social identity theory.
The famous theory explains why as soon as humans are bunched together in groups we start to do odd things: copy other members of our group, favour members of own group over others, look for a leader to worship and fight other groups.
8. Negotiation: 2 Psychological Strategies That Matter Most
Negotiation is one of those activities we often engage in without quite realising it.
Negotiation doesn’t just happen in the boardroom, or when we ask our boss for a raise or down at the market, it happens every time we want to reach an agreement with someone.
In a classic, award-winning series of social psychology experiments, Morgan Deutsch and Robert Krauss investigated two central factors in negotiation: how we communicate with each other and how we use threats.
9. Bystander Effect And The Diffusion Of Responsibility
The bystander effect in social psychology is the surprising finding that the mere presence of other people inhibits our own helping behaviours in an emergency.
The bystander effect social psychology experiments are mentioned in every psychology textbook and often dubbed ‘seminal’.
This famous social psychology experiment on the bystander effect was inspired by the highly publicised murder of Kitty Genovese in 1964.
It found that in some circumstances, the presence of others inhibits people’s helping behaviours — partly because of a phenomenon called diffusion of responsibility.
10. Asch Conformity Experiment: The Power Of Social Pressure
The Asch conformity experiments — some of the most famous every done — were a series of social psychology experiments carried out by noted psychologist Solomon Asch.
The Asch conformity experiment reveals how strongly a person’s opinions are affected by people around them.
In fact, the Asch conformity experiment shows that many of us will deny our own senses just to conform with others.
Author: Dr Jeremy Dean
Psychologist, Jeremy Dean, PhD is the founder and author of PsyBlog. He holds a doctorate in psychology from University College London and two other advanced degrees in psychology. He has been writing about scientific research on PsyBlog since 2004. View all posts by Dr Jeremy Dean
Join the free PsyBlog mailing list. No spam, ever.
15 Famous Experiments and Case Studies in Psychology
Chris Drew (PhD)
Dr. Chris Drew is the founder of the Helpful Professor. He holds a PhD in education and has published over 20 articles in scholarly journals. He is the former editor of the Journal of Learning Development in Higher Education. [Image Descriptor: Photo of Chris]
Learn about our Editorial Process
Psychology has seen thousands upon thousands of research studies over the years. Most of these studies have helped shape our current understanding of human thoughts, behavior, and feelings.
The psychology case studies in this list are considered classic examples of psychological case studies and experiments, which are still being taught in introductory psychology courses up to this day.
Some studies, however, were downright shocking and controversial that you’d probably wonder why such studies were conducted back in the day. Imagine participating in an experiment for a small reward or extra class credit, only to be left scarred for life. These kinds of studies, however, paved the way for a more ethical approach to studying psychology and implementation of research standards such as the use of debriefing in psychology research .
Case Study vs. Experiment
Before we dive into the list of the most famous studies in psychology, let us first review the difference between case studies and experiments.
- It is an in-depth study and analysis of an individual, group, community, or phenomenon. The results of a case study cannot be applied to the whole population, but they can provide insights for further studies.
- It often uses qualitative research methods such as observations, surveys, and interviews.
- It is often conducted in real-life settings rather than in controlled environments.
- An experiment is a type of study done on a sample or group of random participants, the results of which can be generalized to the whole population.
- It often uses quantitative research methods that rely on numbers and statistics.
- It is conducted in controlled environments, wherein some things or situations are manipulated.
See Also: Experimental vs Observational Studies
Famous Experiments in Psychology
1. the marshmallow experiment.
Psychologist Walter Mischel conducted the marshmallow experiment at Stanford University in the 1960s to early 1970s. It was a simple test that aimed to define the connection between delayed gratification and success in life.
The instructions were fairly straightforward: children ages 4-6 were presented a piece of marshmallow on a table and they were told that they would receive a second piece if they could wait for 15 minutes without eating the first marshmallow.
About one-third of the 600 participants succeeded in delaying gratification to receive the second marshmallow. Mischel and his team followed up on these participants in the 1990s, learning that those who had the willpower to wait for a larger reward experienced more success in life in terms of SAT scores and other metrics.
This case study also supported self-control theory , a theory in criminology that holds that people with greater self-control are less likely to end up in trouble with the law!
The classic marshmallow experiment, however, was debunked in a 2018 replication study done by Tyler Watts and colleagues.
This more recent experiment had a larger group of participants (900) and a better representation of the general population when it comes to race and ethnicity. In this study, the researchers found out that the ability to wait for a second marshmallow does not depend on willpower alone but more so on the economic background and social status of the participants.
2. The Bystander Effect
In 1694, Kitty Genovese was murdered in the neighborhood of Kew Gardens, New York. It was told that there were up to 38 witnesses and onlookers in the vicinity of the crime scene, but nobody did anything to stop the murder or call for help.
Such tragedy was the catalyst that inspired social psychologists Bibb Latane and John Darley to formulate the phenomenon called bystander effect or bystander apathy .
Subsequent investigations showed that this story was exaggerated and inaccurate, as there were actually only about a dozen witnesses, at least two of whom called the police. But the case of Kitty Genovese led to various studies that aim to shed light on the bystander phenomenon.
Latane and Darley tested bystander intervention in an experimental study . Participants were asked to answer a questionnaire inside a room, and they would either be alone or with two other participants (who were actually actors or confederates in the study). Smoke would then come out from under the door. The reaction time of participants was tested — how long would it take them to report the smoke to the authorities or the experimenters?
The results showed that participants who were alone in the room reported the smoke faster than participants who were with two passive others. The study suggests that the more onlookers are present in an emergency situation, the less likely someone would step up to help, a social phenomenon now popularly called the bystander effect.
3. Asch Conformity Study
Have you ever made a decision against your better judgment just to fit in with your friends or family? The Asch Conformity Studies will help you understand this kind of situation better.
In this experiment, a group of participants were shown three numbered lines of different lengths and asked to identify the longest of them all. However, only one true participant was present in every group and the rest were actors, most of whom told the wrong answer.
Results showed that the participants went for the wrong answer, even though they knew which line was the longest one in the first place. When the participants were asked why they identified the wrong one, they said that they didn’t want to be branded as strange or peculiar.
This study goes to show that there are situations in life when people prefer fitting in than being right. It also tells that there is power in numbers — a group’s decision can overwhelm a person and make them doubt their judgment.
4. The Bobo Doll Experiment
The Bobo Doll Experiment was conducted by Dr. Albert Bandura, the proponent of social learning theory .
Back in the 1960s, the Nature vs. Nurture debate was a popular topic among psychologists. Bandura contributed to this discussion by proposing that human behavior is mostly influenced by environmental rather than genetic factors.
In the Bobo Doll Experiment, children were divided into three groups: one group was shown a video in which an adult acted aggressively toward the Bobo Doll, the second group was shown a video in which an adult play with the Bobo Doll, and the third group served as the control group where no video was shown.
The children were then led to a room with different kinds of toys, including the Bobo Doll they’ve seen in the video. Results showed that children tend to imitate the adults in the video. Those who were presented the aggressive model acted aggressively toward the Bobo Doll while those who were presented the passive model showed less aggression.
While the Bobo Doll Experiment can no longer be replicated because of ethical concerns, it has laid out the foundations of social learning theory and helped us understand the degree of influence adult behavior has on children.
5. Blue Eye / Brown Eye Experiment
Following the assassination of Martin Luther King Jr. in 1968, third-grade teacher Jane Elliott conducted an experiment in her class. Although not a formal experiment in controlled settings, A Class Divided is a good example of a social experiment to help children understand the concept of racism and discrimination.
The class was divided into two groups: blue-eyed children and brown-eyed children. For one day, Elliott gave preferential treatment to her blue-eyed students, giving them more attention and pampering them with rewards. The next day, it was the brown-eyed students’ turn to receive extra favors and privileges.
As a result, whichever group of students was given preferential treatment performed exceptionally well in class, had higher quiz scores, and recited more frequently; students who were discriminated against felt humiliated, answered poorly in tests, and became uncertain with their answers in class.
This study is now widely taught in sociocultural psychology classes.
6. Stanford Prison Experiment
One of the most controversial and widely-cited studies in psychology is the Stanford Prison Experiment , conducted by Philip Zimbardo at the basement of the Stanford psychology building in 1971. The hypothesis was that abusive behavior in prisons is influenced by the personality traits of the prisoners and prison guards.
The participants in the experiment were college students who were randomly assigned as either a prisoner or a prison guard. The prison guards were then told to run the simulated prison for two weeks. However, the experiment had to be stopped in just 6 days.
The prison guards abused their authority and harassed the prisoners through verbal and physical means. The prisoners, on the other hand, showed submissive behavior. Zimbardo decided to stop the experiment because the prisoners were showing signs of emotional and physical breakdown.
Although the experiment wasn’t completed, the results strongly showed that people can easily get into a social role when others expect them to, especially when it’s highly stereotyped .
7. The Halo Effect
Have you ever wondered why toothpastes and other dental products are endorsed in advertisements by celebrities more often than dentists? The Halo Effect is one of the reasons!
The Halo Effect shows how one favorable attribute of a person can gain them positive perceptions in other attributes. In the case of product advertisements, attractive celebrities are also perceived as intelligent and knowledgeable of a certain subject matter even though they’re not technically experts.
The Halo Effect originated in a classic study done by Edward Thorndike in the early 1900s. He asked military commanding officers to rate their subordinates based on different qualities, such as physical appearance, leadership, dependability, and intelligence.
The results showed that high ratings of a particular quality influences the ratings of other qualities, producing a halo effect of overall high ratings. The opposite also applied, which means that a negative rating in one quality also correlated to negative ratings in other qualities.
Experiments on the Halo Effect came in various formats as well, supporting Thorndike’s original theory. This phenomenon suggests that our perception of other people’s overall personality is hugely influenced by a quality that we focus on.
8. Cognitive Dissonance
There are experiences in our lives when our beliefs and behaviors do not align with each other and we try to justify them in our minds. This is cognitive dissonance , which was studied in an experiment by Leon Festinger and James Carlsmith back in 1959.
In this experiment, participants had to go through a series of boring and repetitive tasks, such as spending an hour turning pegs in a wooden knob. After completing the tasks, they were then paid either $1 or $20 to tell the next participants that the tasks were extremely fun and enjoyable. Afterwards, participants were asked to rate the experiment. Those who were given $1 rated the experiment as more interesting and fun than those who received $20.
The results showed that those who received a smaller incentive to lie experienced cognitive dissonance — $1 wasn’t enough incentive for that one hour of painstakingly boring activity, so the participants had to justify that they had fun anyway.
Famous Case Studies in Psychology
9. little albert.
In 1920, behaviourist theorists John Watson and Rosalie Rayner experimented on a 9-month-old baby to test the effects of classical conditioning in instilling fear in humans.
This was such a controversial study that it gained popularity in psychology textbooks and syllabi because it is a classic example of unethical research studies done in the name of science.
In one of the experiments, Little Albert was presented with a harmless stimulus or object, a white rat, which he wasn’t scared of at first. But every time Little Albert would see the white rat, the researchers would play a scary sound of hammer and steel. After about 6 pairings, Little Albert learned to fear the rat even without the scary sound.
Little Albert developed signs of fear to different objects presented to him through classical conditioning . He even generalized his fear to other stimuli not present in the course of the experiment.
10. Phineas Gage
Phineas Gage is such a celebrity in Psych 101 classes, even though the way he rose to popularity began with a tragic accident. He was a resident of Central Vermont and worked in the construction of a new railway line in the mid-1800s. One day, an explosive went off prematurely, sending a tamping iron straight into his face and through his brain.
Gage survived the accident, fortunately, something that is considered a feat even up to this day. He managed to find a job as a stagecoach after the accident. However, his family and friends reported that his personality changed so much that “he was no longer Gage” (Harlow, 1868).
New evidence on the case of Phineas Gage has since come to light, thanks to modern scientific studies and medical tests. However, there are still plenty of mysteries revolving around his brain damage and subsequent recovery.
11. Anna O.
Anna O., a social worker and feminist of German Jewish descent, was one of the first patients to receive psychoanalytic treatment.
Her real name was Bertha Pappenheim and she inspired much of Sigmund Freud’s works and books on psychoanalytic theory, although they hadn’t met in person. Their connection was through Joseph Breuer, Freud’s mentor when he was still starting his clinical practice.
Anna O. suffered from paralysis, personality changes, hallucinations, and rambling speech, but her doctors could not find the cause. Joseph Breuer was then called to her house for intervention and he performed psychoanalysis, also called the “talking cure”, on her.
Breuer would tell Anna O. to say anything that came to her mind, such as her thoughts, feelings, and childhood experiences. It was noted that her symptoms subsided by talking things out.
However, Breuer later referred Anna O. to the Bellevue Sanatorium, where she recovered and set out to be a renowned writer and advocate of women and children.
12. Patient HM
H.M., or Henry Gustav Molaison, was a severe amnesiac who had been the subject of countless psychological and neurological studies.
Henry was 27 when he underwent brain surgery to cure the epilepsy that he had been experiencing since childhood. In an unfortunate turn of events, he lost his memory because of the surgery and his brain also became unable to store long-term memories.
He was then regarded as someone living solely in the present, forgetting an experience as soon as it happened and only remembering bits and pieces of his past. Over the years, his amnesia and the structure of his brain had helped neuropsychologists learn more about cognitive functions .
Suzanne Corkin, a researcher, writer, and good friend of H.M., recently published a book about his life. Entitled Permanent Present Tense , this book is both a memoir and a case study following the struggles and joys of Henry Gustav Molaison.
13. Chris Sizemore
Chris Sizemore gained celebrity status in the psychology community when she was diagnosed with multiple personality disorder, now known as dissociative identity disorder.
Sizemore has several alter egos, which included Eve Black, Eve White, and Jane. Various papers about her stated that these alter egos were formed as a coping mechanism against the traumatic experiences she underwent in her childhood.
Sizemore said that although she has succeeded in unifying her alter egos into one dominant personality, there were periods in the past experienced by only one of her alter egos. For example, her husband married her Eve White alter ego and not her.
Her story inspired her psychiatrists to write a book about her, entitled The Three Faces of Eve , which was then turned into a 1957 movie of the same title.
14. David Reimer
When David was just 8 months old, he lost his penis because of a botched circumcision operation.
Psychologist John Money then advised Reimer’s parents to raise him as a girl instead, naming him Brenda. His gender reassignment was supported by subsequent surgery and hormonal therapy.
Money described Reimer’s gender reassignment as a success, but problems started to arise as Reimer was growing up. His boyishness was not completely subdued by the hormonal therapy. When he was 14 years old, he learned about the secrets of his past and he underwent gender reassignment to become male again.
Reimer became an advocate for children undergoing the same difficult situation he had been. His life story ended when he was 38 as he took his own life.
15. Kim Peek
Kim Peek was the inspiration behind Rain Man , an Oscar-winning movie about an autistic savant character played by Dustin Hoffman.
The movie was released in 1988, a time when autism wasn’t widely known and acknowledged yet. So it was an eye-opener for many people who watched the film.
In reality, Kim Peek was a non-autistic savant. He was exceptionally intelligent despite the brain abnormalities he was born with. He was like a walking encyclopedia, knowledgeable about travel routes, US zip codes, historical facts, and classical music. He also read and memorized approximately 12,000 books in his lifetime.
This list of experiments and case studies in psychology is just the tip of the iceberg! There are still countless interesting psychology studies that you can explore if you want to learn more about human behavior and dynamics.
You can also conduct your own mini-experiment or participate in a study conducted in your school or neighborhood. Just remember that there are ethical standards to follow so as not to repeat the lasting physical and emotional harm done to Little Albert or the Stanford Prison Experiment participants.
Asch, S. E. (1956). Studies of independence and conformity: I. A minority of one against a unanimous majority. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 70 (9), 1–70. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0093718
Bandura, A., Ross, D., & Ross, S. A. (1961). Transmission of aggression through imitation of aggressive models. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 63 (3), 575–582. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0045925
Elliott, J., Yale University., WGBH (Television station : Boston, Mass.), & PBS DVD (Firm). (2003). A class divided. New Haven, Conn.: Yale University Films.
Festinger, L., & Carlsmith, J. M. (1959). Cognitive consequences of forced compliance. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 58 (2), 203–210. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0041593
Haney, C., Banks, W. C., & Zimbardo, P. G. (1973). A study of prisoners and guards in a simulated prison. Naval Research Review , 30 , 4-17.
Latane, B., & Darley, J. M. (1968). Group inhibition of bystander intervention in emergencies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 10 (3), 215–221. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0026570
Mischel, W. (2014). The Marshmallow Test: Mastering self-control. Little, Brown and Co.
Thorndike, E. (1920) A Constant Error in Psychological Ratings. Journal of Applied Psychology , 4 , 25-29. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0071663
Watson, J. B., & Rayner, R. (1920). Conditioned emotional reactions. Journal of experimental psychology , 3 (1), 1.
- Chris Drew (PhD) https://helpfulprofessor.com/author/chris-drew-phd-2/ Free Social Skills Worksheets
- Chris Drew (PhD) https://helpfulprofessor.com/author/chris-drew-phd-2/ 10 Reasons you’re Perpetually Single
- Chris Drew (PhD) https://helpfulprofessor.com/author/chris-drew-phd-2/ 20 Montessori Toddler Bedrooms (Design Inspiration)
- Chris Drew (PhD) https://helpfulprofessor.com/author/chris-drew-phd-2/ 21 Montessori Homeschool Setups
Leave a Comment Cancel Reply
Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *
Understanding the Importance of Case Studies in Psychology
Case studies play a crucial role in the field of psychology, providing researchers with valuable insights into human behavior and mental processes. From providing real-life examples to generating hypotheses and analyzing rare phenomena, case studies offer a deep dive into the complexities of the human mind.
But how are these studies conducted, and what are the different types? In this article, we will explore the significance of case studies in psychology, their methodology, and the various types that researchers utilize in their work.
- Case studies provide real-life examples to support theories and concepts in psychology.
- They allow for in-depth analysis of individual experiences, thoughts, and behaviors.
- Case studies help generate hypotheses and provide insight into rare phenomena, making them valuable in research.
- 1 What Are Case Studies in Psychology?
- 2.1 Provide Real-life Examples
- 2.2 Allow for In-depth Analysis
- 2.3 Help in Generating Hypotheses
- 2.4 Provide Insight into Rare Phenomena
- 2.5 Can be Used in Ethically Sensitive Situations
- 3.1 Selecting a Case
- 3.2 Collecting Data
- 3.3 Analyzing Data
- 3.4 Reporting Findings
- 4.1 Descriptive Case Studies
- 4.2 Exploratory Case Studies
- 4.3 Explanatory Case Studies
- 4.4 Collective Case Studies
- 4.5 Single-Case Studies
- 4.6 Multiple-Case Studies
- 5.1 What are case studies in psychology?
- 5.2 Why are case studies important in psychology?
- 5.3 How do case studies help in understanding psychology?
- 5.4 What are the benefits of using case studies in psychology?
- 5.5 Can case studies be used to generalize findings?
- 5.6 What are the limitations of using case studies in psychology?
What Are Case Studies in Psychology?
Case studies in psychology refer to detailed investigations of a single individual, group, or event to explore underlying principles and behaviors.
These studies are conducted to gain a deeper understanding of specific psychological phenomena and provide valuable insights into the complexities of human behavior. By focusing on a particular subject, researchers are able to examine real-world scenarios in depth, uncovering unique aspects that may not be captured through larger-scale studies.
The purpose of case studies in psychology is not only to analyze specific cases but also to draw broader conclusions and develop theories that can contribute to the advancement of psychological knowledge. They serve as a crucial tool in the research process, allowing researchers to examine complex interactions and variables within a controlled framework.
Why Are Case Studies Important in Psychology?
Case studies hold significant importance in psychology due to their ability to provide detailed insights into individual behavior and psychological phenomena.
Through case studies , psychologists can delve deep into the unique circumstances surrounding a particular person or group, shedding light on complex psychological processes that may not be easily observable in large-scale research.
These in-depth examinations enable researchers to explore rare phenomena that may not be commonly encountered in everyday life, offering a valuable opportunity to study and understand the complexities of human behavior.
Case studies play a crucial role in generating hypotheses for further investigation, laying the groundwork for more extensive research studies.
Provide Real-life Examples
Case studies in psychology offer real-life examples that allow researchers and practitioners to apply theoretical concepts to actual scenarios, a practice famously exemplified by Sigmund Freud in his clinical work.
Freud’s use of case studies, such as the famous ‘Anna O.’ case, revolutionized how psychologists understand and treat mental disorders.
These in-depth investigations delve into an individual’s thoughts, emotions, and behaviors, providing rich data for analyzing psychological principles in action.
By examining specific cases, professionals can gain insights into varying mental health conditions, like schizophrenia, PTSD, or phobias.
Allow for In-depth Analysis
Case studies enable researchers to conduct in-depth analyses of complex psychological phenomena, facilitating detailed data collection and rigorous analysis methods.
By delving deeply into individual cases, researchers can gather rich, detailed information that may not be accessible through other research methods.
Data collection techniques such as interviews, observations, and archival research are commonly employed to gather comprehensive data.
Analytical approaches such as thematic analysis, content analysis, and grounded theory are then utilized to interpret the gathered information and derive meaningful insights.
Through this intricate process, researchers can uncover nuanced patterns, behaviors, and underlying psychological dynamics, leading to qualitative research that offers valuable insights into human behavior and cognition.
Help in Generating Hypotheses
Case studies play a key role in generating hypotheses within psychology by providing rich qualitative data that can inform diagnostic processes and research directions.
These in-depth analyses of individual cases allow researchers to delve into specific behaviors, experiences, and psychological phenomena, offering detailed insights that can lead to the formulation of new theories or test existing hypotheses.
- By examining unique cases, psychologists can identify patterns, outliers, and uncommon manifestations that expand their understanding of mental health conditions and human behavior.
- The flexibility of case study research methods enables researchers to adapt their approach to each unique case, accommodating the complex and intricate nature of psychological disorders.
This nuanced exploration of individual experiences and symptoms contributes significantly to the development of accurate diagnostic criteria and effective treatment strategies, enhancing the overall quality of psychological research and clinical practice.
Provide Insight into Rare Phenomena
Case studies offer valuable insights into rare psychological phenomena that may not be easily captured through traditional research methods, such as the application of cognitive behavioral therapy in exceptional cases.
These in-depth examinations of individual cases allow researchers to delve deeper into unique psychological abnormalities that may defy standard treatment strategies. For instance, rare cases of dissociative identity disorder have shown remarkable progress with the implementation of cognitive behavioral therapy , shedding light on the potential effectiveness of this unconventional approach. Through detailed narratives of patient experiences and therapeutic interventions, case studies provide a rich source of information for psychologists and therapists alike, offering a nuanced understanding of complex psychological conditions.
Can be Used in Ethically Sensitive Situations
Case studies are instrumental in addressing ethically sensitive situations within psychology, ensuring the competence and adherence to ethical guidelines in clinical practice and research.
In the realm of clinical settings, it is imperative for psychologists to uphold the highest ethical standards when dealing with delicate ethical dilemmas that may arise. By diving into case studies, professionals can gain valuable insights into the complexities of ethical decision-making processes. For a better understanding of the importance of case studies in psychology, you can refer to this external link .
The application of ethical principles plays a fundamental role in safeguarding the welfare of clients and maintaining trust in the therapeutic relationship. Ethical considerations serve as the moral compass guiding psychologists in navigating intricate scenarios that may challenge their professional integrity.
How Are Case Studies Conducted in Psychology?
Case studies in psychology are conducted through a systematic process involving the selection of a case, data collection, thorough analysis, and comprehensive reporting of findings.
Regarding selecting a case for a psychological study, researchers typically look for rich and detailed examples that can provide valuable insights into specific phenomena or conditions. This selection process often involves careful consideration of factors such as uniqueness, relevance to the research question, and the availability of information.
Data collection methods in psychological case studies can vary, ranging from structured interviews and observations to psychological tests and document analysis. These methods aim to gather comprehensive and reliable data that can offer a deep understanding of the case under investigation.
Once the data is collected, researchers employ various analytical approaches, such as thematic analysis, content analysis, or grounded theory, to identify patterns, themes, and relationships within the data. These analytical techniques help in uncovering significant findings and drawing meaningful conclusions.
Selecting a Case
The initial step in conducting a case study in psychology involves selecting an individual case with a compelling personal history that aligns with the research objectives.
It is paramount to consider the individual’s background, experiences, and unique attributes when choosing a case.
- Assessing the personal history of the case subject can provide valuable insights into potential factors contributing to their current situation.
- Ensuring that the research objectives are in harmony with the characteristics of the chosen case helps maintain relevance and coherence in the study.
- Each case has its own complexities, challenges, and dynamics that can offer rich material for analysis and interpretation.
Therefore, a thorough evaluation of the personal history, alignment with research goals, and distinct qualities of the case individual are crucial steps in the selection process for a psychological case study.
Collecting Data
Data collection in psychological case studies involves various methods such as observational studies , archival record reviews, and direct interviews to gather comprehensive information about the case.
Observational studies, one of the primary data collection techniques in psychological case studies, involve the systematic observation of individuals in their natural environment to understand their behavior and interactions. Researchers closely analyze and document these observations to gain insights into the case under study.
Archival record reviews play a crucial role in accessing historical information related to the case. Researchers sift through records such as medical reports, school transcripts, or legal documents to gather valuable data that provides context and background for the case.
Interview methods offer researchers the opportunity to delve deeper into the case by directly interacting with the individuals involved. Through structured or semi-structured interviews, researchers can gather detailed accounts, emotions, and perspectives, enriching the understanding of the case.
Analyzing Data
Data analysis in psychological case studies involves rigorous examination methods such as interpretative phenomenological analysis to derive meaningful insights from collected information.
This approach aims to uncover underlying themes and patterns within the data, allowing researchers to delve deep into the subjective experiences of individuals studied. Through thematic coding practices, researchers categorize and organize data based on recurring themes or concepts, facilitating a systematic analysis process. By employing analytical rigor and attention to detail, researchers can extract valuable insights that contribute to a more profound understanding of the psychological phenomena under investigation.
Reporting Findings
Reporting findings in psychological case studies follows established guidelines such as the APA format, ensuring clear and concise documentation of the research outcomes and clinical observations.
Following the APA format standards is crucial in the field of psychology as it provides a structured framework for presenting research data and insights. By adhering to these guidelines, researchers can enhance the credibility and reproducibility of their findings, fostering transparency in the scientific community.
The meticulous reporting of results not only aids in the replication of studies but also facilitates the integration of new knowledge into existing theories and practices. Maintaining consistency in clinical documentation allows for effective communication among professionals, promoting collaborative efforts in advancing psychological research.
What Are the Different Types of Case Studies in Psychology?
In psychology, various types of case studies exist, including descriptive case studies that aim to provide detailed accounts and explanatory case studies that focus on understanding underlying psychological mechanisms.
Descriptive case studies often involve extensive qualitative data collection through methods such as interviews, observations, and document analysis to present a rich narrative of an individual’s experiences or behaviors.
On the other hand, explanatory case studies delve deeper into the causal relationships between variables, seeking to uncover the reasons behind certain psychological phenomena or behaviors.
While descriptive studies offer a comprehensive portrayal, explanatory studies strive to unravel the ‘why’ and ‘how’ aspects, shedding light on the mechanisms at play in complex psychological processes.
Descriptive Case Studies
Descriptive case studies in psychology involve detailed observations and analyses of individual cases to provide comprehensive insights into behavior and psychological processes.
Observational research methods play a crucial role in conducting descriptive case studies, allowing researchers to directly observe and document the behaviors and responses of the individuals under study. These methods often involve naturalistic observations in real-life settings, providing rich and nuanced data for analysis. Researchers often employ thematic coding practices to categorize and analyze the qualitative data collected during these observations, identifying recurring themes and patterns within the behavior of the case subjects.
Exploratory Case Studies
Exploratory case studies in psychology aim to delve into new areas of research, employing thematic coding to identify patterns and themes within the collected data.
Thematic coding, a crucial methodology in these studies, involves categorizing data based on recurring themes and concepts that emerge during analysis. This process allows researchers to uncover hidden connections and gain deeper insight into the phenomena under investigation.
Longitudinal studies, often utilized in exploratory research, provide a temporal perspective by tracking changes and developments over an extended period. This longitudinal approach enables researchers to observe how patterns evolve over time, offering a more comprehensive understanding of complex psychological processes.
Explanatory Case Studies
Explanatory case studies in psychology focus on understanding the underlying psychological phenomena through interpretative phenomenological analysis, uncovering the complexities of individual behavior.
By utilizing interpretative phenomenological analysis methods, researchers can delve deep into the subjective experiences and perceptions of participants, providing rich insights into the intricate workings of the human mind. These case studies serve as valuable tools for theory testing approaches, allowing psychologists to explore the applicability and validity of existing theories in real-world contexts.
The theoretical interpretations derived from these studies help in advancing our understanding of psychological processes, shedding light on factors that influence behavior, emotions, and cognition. Through detailed qualitative analysis, researchers can capture the nuances and subtleties of individual differences and diverse psychological phenomena, contributing to the broader field of psychology.
Collective Case Studies
Collective case studies in psychology involve the examination of multiple cases or a group of individuals over an extended period, often utilizing longitudinal study designs to capture developmental changes.
The primary goal of collective case studies is to provide a comprehensive understanding of specific phenomena by observing patterns across different cases. By analyzing these cases simultaneously, researchers can gain insights into shared experiences, unique outcomes, and variations that might occur within the group.
Longitudinal study frameworks in collective case studies allow researchers to track changes over time, providing valuable data on long-term behavioral patterns and psychological development.
Cross-sectional comparisons, on the other hand, enable researchers to compare different cases at a specific point in time, helping identify similarities, differences, and potential factors influencing group dynamics.
Single-Case Studies
Single-case studies in psychology focus on individual cases, allowing for a detailed examination of unique behaviors and psychological patterns while addressing potential researcher biases.
By diving into a single individual’s experience, researchers can gain in-depth insights into specific behaviors and responses. This approach enables a focused analysis of the complexities that may not be observable in larger samples, leading to a nuanced understanding of psychological phenomena.
Scientific rigor plays a crucial role in single-case studies, requiring meticulous observation, documentation, and interpretation to uphold the validity and reliability of findings.
Attention to ethical considerations is paramount in these studies, ensuring the well-being and autonomy of the participant while maintaining the integrity of the research process. Researchers must also navigate the limitations inherent in single-case studies, such as generalizability issues and the need for robust experimental controls to establish causal relationships.
Multiple-Case Studies
Multiple-case studies in psychology involve the comparison of several cases to identify commonalities, differences, and patterns, allowing for generalizations within specific contexts.
When conducting multiple-case studies, researchers typically aim to extend beyond individual case analyses to explore overarching themes and patterns that may apply to a larger population or phenomenon.
By utilizing comparative analysis, researchers can delve into the nuances of each case, observing the interplay of various factors and variables that contribute to specific outcomes.
Through this method, researchers can test existing theories, refine hypotheses, and develop new insights that contribute to the broader understanding of complex psychological phenomena.
Frequently Asked Questions
What are case studies in psychology.
Case studies in psychology involve in-depth analysis and examination of an individual, group, or event, usually to understand underlying psychological principles and phenomena.
Why are case studies important in psychology?
Case studies provide detailed and specific information about an individual or group, allowing researchers to gain a deep understanding of complex psychological processes and behaviors.
How do case studies help in understanding psychology?
Case studies help in understanding psychology by providing real-world examples of psychological theories and concepts, making them easier to comprehend and apply.
What are the benefits of using case studies in psychology?
Case studies offer a rich source of data, provide insights into rare or unusual cases, and allow for the exploration of complex and multifaceted phenomena in a real-world context.
Can case studies be used to generalize findings?
No, case studies are not meant to be used for generalization as they focus on a specific individual or group and do not represent the entire population. However, they can be used to generate hypotheses for future research.
What are the limitations of using case studies in psychology?
Some limitations of case studies include potential bias from the researcher, difficulty in replicating results, and lack of control over variables. Additionally, they may not be generalizable to the broader population.
Rachel Liu, a social psychologist, examines the influence of societal and cultural factors on individual behavior. Her research has spanned topics from group dynamics and social identity to the psychological impacts of social media. Rachel’s writing aims to illuminate the ways in which social environments shape our thoughts, feelings, and actions, offering insights into improving interpersonal relationships and fostering inclusive communities.
Similar Posts
Unlocking the Power of Multiple Regression in Psychology: Statistical Analysis and Prediction
The article was last updated by Ethan Clarke on February 6, 2024. Have you ever wondered how researchers in psychology are able to predict outcomes…
Understanding the Purpose of Research Studies in Psychology
The article was last updated by Dr. Henry Foster on February 5, 2024. Research studies in psychology play a crucial role in understanding human behavior,…
Generalizing Conclusions in Psychology: Best Practices
The article was last updated by Dr. Emily Tan on February 4, 2024. Generalizing conclusions in psychology is a crucial aspect of research, but it…
Understanding the Significance of Matched Pairs Design in Psychology
The article was last updated by Alicia Rhodes on February 4, 2024. Matched pairs design is a crucial methodology in psychology that allows researchers to…
The Significance of Operational Definitions in Psychology
The article was last updated by Lena Nguyen on February 5, 2024. Operational definitions play a crucial role in the field of psychology, providing clear…
Understanding Respondent Behavior in Psychological Studies
The article was last updated by Sofia Alvarez on February 4, 2024. Respondent behavior plays a crucial role in psychological studies, shedding light on how…
- Even more »
Account Options
- Try the new Google Books
- Advanced Book Search
- Barnes&Noble.com
- Books-A-Million
- Find in a library
- All sellers »
Get Textbooks on Google Play
Rent and save from the world's largest eBookstore. Read, highlight, and take notes, across web, tablet, and phone.
Go to Google Play Now »
Selected pages
Other editions - View all
Common terms and phrases, about the author (2018).
Thomas Heinzen is a full time teacher/researcher at William Paterson University of New Jersey. He conducted evaluation research for the Rockefeller College of Public Affairs and Policy, consulted on preventing tractor roll-overs at the New York Center of Agricultural Health and Medicine in Cooperstown, New York, and conducted statistical analyses for a temporary commission on homeless veterans in New York State. He invests in students' lives by mentoring student articles and presentations, designing a novel internship program at his university, and creating a lab that tests game-based interventions with the aim to increase rates of college completion among at-risk students.
Wind Goodfriend has been teaching psychology at Buena Vista University, a Midwestern liberal arts school for twelve years. Wind is a three-time Faculty of the Year award winner. She became Assistant Dean of Graduate Studies in 2017. She also serves as the co-director of the trauma advocacy program and volunteers as the chief research officer for the Institute for the Prevention of Relationship Violence. Wind has written 13 book chapters on psychology in pop culture, covering topics including Game of Thrones, Wonder Woman, Doctor Who, Star Trek, and more. She has developed a wide variety of undergraduate courses including special topics classes such as Psychology of Colonialism, Psychology in Popular Film, and Relationship Violence. She received her B.A. from Buena Vista University and both her Master’s and Ph.D. in Social Psychology from Purdue University.
Bibliographic information
Advertisement
A Case for the Case Study: How and Why They Matter
- Original Paper
- Published: 06 June 2017
- Volume 45 , pages 189–200, ( 2017 )
Cite this article
- Jeffrey Longhofer 1 ,
- Jerry Floersch 1 &
- Eric Hartmann 2
4234 Accesses
3 Altmetric
Explore all metrics
In this special issue we have asked the contributors to make a case for the case study. The guest editors, Jeffrey Longhofer, Jerry Floersch and Eric Hartmann, intergrate ideas from across the disciplines to explore the complexties of case study methods and theory. In education, Gary Thomas explores the importance of ethnographic case studies in understanding the relationships among schools, teachers, and students. Lance Dodes and Josh Dodes use the case study to articulate a psychoanalytic approach to addiction. In policy and generalist practice, Nancy Cartwright and Jeremy Hardie elaborate a model for a case-by-case approach to prediction and the swampy ground prediction serves up to practitioners. Christian Salas and Oliver Turnbull persuasively write about the role of the case study in neuro-psychoanalysis and illustrate it with a case vignette. In political science, Sanford Schram argues for a bottom up and ethnographic approach to studying policy implementation by describing a case of a home ownership program in Philadelphia. Eric Hartman queers the case study by articulating its role in deconstructing normative explanations of sexuality. In applied psychology, Daniel Fishman describes a comprehensive applied psychology perspective on the paradigmatic case study. Richard Miller and Miriam Jaffe offer us important ways of thinking about writing the case study and the use of multi-media. Each contributor brings a unique perspective to the use of the case study in their field, yet they share practical and philosophical assumptions.
This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.
Access this article
Subscribe and save.
- Get 10 units per month
- Download Article/Chapter or eBook
- 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
- Cancel anytime
Price excludes VAT (USA) Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.
Instant access to the full article PDF.
Rent this article via DeepDyve
Institutional subscriptions
Similar content being viewed by others
Psychoanalysis, social science and the Tavistock tradition
The history and future of the sociology of therapy: a review and a research agenda, a qualitative analysis and eleven-factor typology of hypothesized factors encouraging or discouraging the development of problem solving courts in various countries.
Aastrup, J., & Halldórsson, Á (2008). Epistemological role of case studies in logistics: A critical realist perspective. International Journal of Physical Distribution & Logistics Management, 38 (10), 746–763.
Article Google Scholar
Abbott, A. (1992). What do cases do? Some notes on activity in sociological analysis. In C. C. Ragin, & H. S. Becker (Eds.), What is a case?: Exploring the foundations of social inquiry (pp. 53–82). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Google Scholar
Ahbel-Rappe, K. (2009). “After a long pause”: How to read Dora as history. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 57 (3), 595–629.
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Alfonso, C. A. (2002). Frontline—writing psychoanalytic case reports: Safeguarding privacy while preserving integrity. Journal of the American Academy of Psychoanalysis and Dynamic Psychiatry, 30 (2), 165–172.
Altstein, R. (2016). Finding words: How the process and products of psychoanalytic writing can channel the therapeutic action of the very treatment it sets out to describe. Psychoanalytic Perspectives, 13 (1), 51–70.
Anderson, W. (2013). The case of the archive. Critical Inquiry, 39 (3), 532–547.
Antommaria, A. (2004). Do as I say, not as I do: Why bioethicists should seek informed consent for some case studies. Hastings Center Report, 34 (3), 28–34.
Archer, M. S. (2010). Routine, reflexivity, and realism. Sociological Theory , 28 (3), 272–303.
Aron, L. (2000). Ethical considerations in the writing of psychoanalytic case histories. Psychoanalytic Dialogues, 10 (2), 231–245.
Aron, L. (2016). Ethical considerations in psychoanalytic writing revisited. Psychoanalytic Perspectives, 13 (3), 267–290.
Barth, M., & Thomas, I. (2012). Synthesising case-study research: Ready for the next step? Environmental Education Research, 18 (6), 751–764.
Benner, P. (1982). From novice to expert. The American Journal of Nursing, 82 (3), 402–407.
PubMed Google Scholar
Benner, P. (2000a). The wisdom of our practice. The American Journal of Nursing, 100 (10), 99–105.
Benner, P. (2000b). The roles of embodiment, emotion and lifeworld for rationality and agency in nursing practice. Nursing Philosophy, 1 (1), 5–19.
Benner, P. (2004). Using the Dreyfus model of skill acquisition to describe and interpret skill acquisition and clinical judgment in nursing practice and education. Bulletin of Science, Technology & Society, 24 (3), 188–199.
Bennett, A., & Elman, C. (2006). Qualitative research: Recent developments in case study methods. Annual Review of Political Science, 9 , 455–476.
Bennett, A., & Elman, C. (2007). Case study methods in the international relations subfield. Comparative Political Studies, 40 (2), 170–195.
Bergene, A. (2007). Towards a critical realist comparative methodology. Journal of Critical Realism, 3 (1), 5–27.
Berlant, L. (2007). On the case. Critical Inquiry, 33 (4), 663–672.
Bernstein, S. B. (2008a). Writing about the psychoanalytic process. Psychoanalytic Inquiry, 28 (4), 433–449.
Bernstein, S. B. (2008b). Writing, rewriting, and working through. Psychoanalytic Inquiry, 28 (4), 450–464.
Blechner, M. (2012). Confidentiality: Against disguise, for consent. Psychotherapy, 49 (1), 16–18.
Bornstein, R. F. (2007). Nomothetic psychoanalysis. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 24 (4), 590–602.
Bourdieu, P. (1990). The scholastic point of view. Cultural Anthropology, 5 (4), 380–391.
Bourdieu, P. (2000). Pascalian meditations . Stanford: Stanford University Press.
Boyce, N. (2015). Dora in the 21st century. Lancet, 386 (9997), 948–949.
Brandell, J., & Varkas, T. (2010). Narrative case studies. In B. Thyer (Ed.), The handbook of social work research methods (Chap. 20, pp. 375–396). Los Angeles: SAGE
Bunch, W. H., & Dvonch, V. M. (2000). Moral decisions regarding innovation: The case method. Clinical Orthopaedics and Related Research, 378 , 44–49.
Burawoy, M. (1998). The extended case method. Sociological theory, 16 (1), 4–33.
Campbell, D. T. (1975). “Degrees of freedom” and the case study. Comparative Political Studies, 8 (2), 178–193.
Carlson, J. (2010). Commentary: Writing about clients—ethical and professional issues in clinical case reports. Counseling and Values, 54 (2), 154–157.
Cartwright, N. (2007). Are RCTs the gold standard? BioSocieties, 2 (1), 11–20.
Cartwright, N. (2011). A philosopher’s view of the long road from RCTs to effectiveness. Lancet, 377 (9775), 1400–1401.
Charlton, B. G., & Walston, F. (1998). Individual case studies in clinical research. Journal of Evaluation in Clinical Practice, 4 (2), 147–155.
Colombo, D., & Michels, R. (2007). Can (should) case reports be written for research use? Psychoanalytic Inquiry, 27 (5), 640–649.
Damousi, J., Lang, B., & Sutton, K. (Eds.) (2015). Case studies and the dissemination of knowledge . London: Routledge Press.
Desmet, M., Meganck, R., Seybert, C., Willemsen, J., Geerardyn, F., Declercq, F., & Schindler, I. (2012). Psychoanalytic single cases published in ISI-ranked journals: The construction of an online archive. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 82 (2), 120–121.
Dobson, P. J. (2001). Longitudinal case research: A critical realist perspective. Systemic Practice and Action Research, 14 (3), 283–296.
Dreyfus, H. L. (2008). On the internet . London: Routledge Press.
Dreyfus, H. L., & Dreyfus, S. E. (2005). Peripheral vision expertise in real world contexts. Organization Studies, 26 (5), 779–792.
Easton, G. (2010). Critical realism in case study research. Industrial Marketing Management, 39 (1), 118–128.
Edelson, M. (1985). The hermeneutic turn and the single case study in psychoanalysis. Psychoanalysis & Contemporary Thought, 8 , 567–614.
Feagin, J. R., Orum, A. M., & Sjoberg, G. (Eds.) (1991). A case for the case study . Chapel Hill: University of North Carolina Press.
Ferguson, H. (2016). Researching social work practice close up: Using ethnographic and mobile methods to understand encounters between social workers, children and families. British Journal of Social Work, 46 (1), 153–168.
Fisher, M. A. (2013). The ethics of conditional confidentiality: A practice model for mental health professionals . Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Book Google Scholar
Fleischman, J. (2002). Phineas Gage: A gruesome but true story about brain science . New York: Houghton Mifflin.
Floersch, J. (2000). Reading the case record: The oral and written narratives of social workers. Social Service Review, 74 (2), 169–192.
Floersch, J. (2002). Meds, money, and manners: The case management of severe mental illness . Columbia: Columbia University Press.
Floersch, J., & Longhofer, J. (2016). Social work and the scholastic fallacy. Investigacao Em Trabalho Social, 3 , 71–91. https://www.isssp.pt/si/web_base.gera_pagina?p_pagina=21798 .
Florek, A. G., & Dellavalle, R. P. (2016). Case reports in medical education: A platform for training medical students, residents, and fellows in scientific writing and critical thinking. Journal of Medical Case Reports, 10 (1), 1.
Flyvbjerg, B. (2001). Making social science matter: Why social inquiry fails and how it can succeed again . Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Flyvbjerg, B. (2006). Five misunderstandings about case-study research. Qualitative Inquiry, 12 (2), 219–245.
Flyvbjerg, B., Landman, T., & Schram, S. (Eds.). (2012). Real social science: Applied phronesis . Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Forrester, J. (1996). If p, then what? Thinking in cases. History of the Human Sciences, 9 (3), 1–25.
Forrester, J. (2007). On Kuhn’s case: Psychoanalysis and the paradigm. Critical Inquiry, 33 (4), 782–819.
Foucault, M. (1994). The birth of the clinic: An archaeology of medical perception . London: Routledge Press.
Freeman, W. J. (2000). How brains make up their minds . New York: Columbia University Press.
Freud, S. (1905). Fragment of an analysis of a case of hysteria. Standard Edition , 7 , 7–122.
Freud, S. (1909a). Analysis of a phobia in a five-year-old boy. Standard Edition , 10 , 5–147.
Freud, S. (1909b). Notes upon a case of obsessional neurosis. Standard Edition , 10 , 151–318.
Freud, S. (1911). Psycho-analytic notes on an autobiographical account of a case of paranoia (dementia paranoides). Standard Edition , 12 , 9–79.
Freud, S. (1918). From the history of an infantile neurosis. Standard Edition , 17 , 7–122.
George, A. L. & Bennett, A. (2004). Case studies and theory development in the social sciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Gerring, J. (2004). What is a case study and what is it good for? American Political Science Review, 98 (02), 341–354.
Gerring, J. (2007a). Case study research: Principles and practices . New York: Cambridge University Press.
Gerring, J. (2007b). Is there a (viable) crucial-case method? Comparative Political Studies, 40 (3), 231–253.
Gilgun, J. F. (1994). A case for case studies in social work research. Social Work, 39 , 371–380.
Gottdiener, W. H., & Suh, J. J. (2012). Expanding the single-case study: A proposed psychoanalytic research program. The Psychoanalytic Review, 99 (1), 81–102.
Gupta, M. (2007). Does evidence-based medicine apply to psychiatry? Theoretical Medicine and Bioethics, 28 (2), 103–120.
Gupta, M. (2014). Is evidence-based psychiatry ethical? Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Gwande, A. (2014). Being mortal: Medicine and what matters most in the end . New York: Metropolitan Books.
Haas, L. (2001). Phineas Gage and the science of brain localisation. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 71 (6), 761.
Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Hacking, I. (1999). The social construction of what? Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Haggis, T. (2008). ‘Knowledge must be contextual’: Some possible implications of complexity and dynamic systems theories for educational research. Educational Philosophy and Theory, 40 (1), 158–176.
Hare-Mustin, R. T. (1983). An appraisal of the relationship between women and psychotherapy: 80 years after the case of Dora. American Psychologist, 38 (5), 593–601.
Held, B. S. (2009). The logic of case-study methodology. Pragmatic Case Studies in Psychotherapy, 5 (3), 90–100.
Hoffman, I. Z. (2009). Doublethinking our way to “scientific” legitimacy: The desiccation of human experience. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 57 (5), 1043–1069.
Hollan, D., & Throop, C. J. (2008). Whatever happened to empathy?: Introduction. Ethos, 36 (4), 385–401.
Hurwitz, B. (2011). Clinical cases and case reports: Boundaries and porosities. The Case and the Canon. Anomalies, Discontinuities, Metaphors Between Science and Literature , 45–57.
Ioannidis, J. P., Haidich, A. B., & Lau, J. (2001). Any casualties in the clash of randomized and observational evidence? British Medical Journal, 322 , 879–880.
Iosifides, T. (2012). Migration research between positivistic scientism and relativism: A critical realist way out. In C. Vargas-Silva (Ed.), Handbook of research methods in migration (pp. 26–49). Cheltenham: Edward Elgar Publishing.
Kächele, H., Schachter, J., & Thomä, H. (2011). From psychoanalytic narrative to empirical single case research: Implications for psychoanalytic practice (vol. 30). New York: Taylor & Francis.
Kantrowitz, J. L. (2004). Writing about patients: I. Ways of protecting confidentiality and analysts’ conflicts over choice of method. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 52 (1), 69–99.
Ketokivi, M., & Choi, T. (2014). Renaissance of case research as a scientific method. Journal of Operations Management, 32 (5), 232–240.
Kitchin, R. (2014a). Big data, new epistemologies and paradigm shifts. Big Data & Society, 1 (1), 1–12.
Kitchin, R. (2014b). The data revolution: Big data, open data, data infrastructures and their consequences . Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Press.
Kitchin, R., & Lauriault, T. P. (2015). Small data in the era of big data. GeoJournal, 80 (4), 463–475.
Koenig, G. (2009). Realistic evaluation and case studies stretching the potential. Evaluation, 15 (1), 9–30.
Lahire, B. (2011). The plural actor . Cambridge: Polity.
Leong, S. M. (1985). Metatheory and metamethodology in marketing, a lakatosian reconstruction. Journal of Marketing, 49 (4), 23–40.
Levy, J. S. (2008). Case studies: Types, designs, and logics of inference. Conflict Management and Peace Science, 25 (1), 1–18.
Longhofer, J., & Floersch, J. (2012). The coming crisis in social work: Some thoughts on social work and science. Research on Social Work Practice, 22 (5), 499–519.
Longhofer, J., & Floersch, J. (2014). Values in a science of social work: Values-informed research and research-informed values. Research on Social Work Practice, 24 (5), 527–534.
Longhofer, J., Floersch, J., & Hoy, J. (2013). Qualitative methods for practice research. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Luyten, P., Corveleyn, J., & Blatt, S. J. (2006). Minding the gap between positivism and hermeneutics in psychoanalytic research. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 54 (2), 571–610.
Macmillan, M. (2000). Restoring phineas gage: A 150th retrospective. Journal of the History of the Neurosciences, 9 (1), 46–66.
Mahoney, J., Kimball, E., & Koivu, K. L. (2009). The logic of historical explanation in the social sciences. Comparative Political Studies, 42 (1), 114–146.
Marchal, B., Westhorp, G., Wong, G., Van Belle, S., Greenhalgh, T., Kegels, G., & Pawson, R. (2013). Realist RCTs of complex interventions: An oxymoron. Social Science & Medicine, 94 (12), 124–128.
Maroglin, L. (1997). Under the cover of kindness. The invention of social work . Charlottesville: University Press of Virginia.
McKeown, A. (2015). Critical realism and empirical bioethics: A methodological exposition. Health Care Analysis, 1–21. doi: 10.1007/s10728-015-0290-2 .
McLeod, J. (2010). Case study research in counseling and psychotherapy . Thousand Oaks, LA: Sage Publications.
McLeod, J. (2015). Reading case studies to inform therapeutic practice. In Psychotherapie forum (vol. 20, No. 1–2, pp. 3–9). Vienna: Springer.
McLeod, J., & Balamoutsou, S. (1996). Representing narrative process in therapy: Qualitative analysis of a single case. Counseling Psychology Quarterly, 9 (1), 61–76.
Mearsheimer, J. J., & Walt, S. M. (2013). Leaving theory behind: Why simplistic hypothesis testing is bad for international relations. European Journal of International Relations, 19 (3), 427–457.
Michels, R. (2000). The case history. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 48 (2), 355–375.
Miller, E. (2009). Writing about patients: What clinical and literary writers share. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 57 (5), 1097–1120.
Mingers, J. (2004). Realizing information systems: Critical realism as an underpinning philosophy for information systems. Information and Organization, 14 (2), 87–103.
Mishna, F. (2004). A qualitative study of bullying from multiple perspectives. Children & Schools, 26 (4), 234–247.
Mittelstadt, B. D., Allo, P., Taddeo, M., Wachter, S., & Floridi, L. (2016). The ethics of algorithms: Mapping the debate. Big Data & Society , 3(2), 2053951716679679
Morgan, M. S. (2012). Case studies: One observation or many? Justification or discovery? Philosophy of Science, 79 (5), 667–677.
Naiburg, S. (2015). Structure and spontaneity in clinical prose: A writer’s guide for psychoanalysts and psychotherapists . London: Routledge Press.
Nissen, T., & Wynn, R. (2014a). The clinical case report: A review of its merits and limitations. BMC Research Notes, 7 (1), 1–7.
Nissen, T., & Wynn, R. (2014b). The history of the case report: A selective review. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine Open, 5 (4), 1–5. doi: 10.1177/2054270414523410 .
O’Neil, C. (2016). Weapons of math destruction: How big data increases inequality and threatens democracy . New York: Crown Publishing Group.
Payne, S., Field, D., Rolls, L., Hawker, S., & Kerr, C. (2007). Case study research methods in end-of-life care: Reflections on three studies. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 58 (3), 236–245.
Perry, C. (1998). Processes of a case study methodology for postgraduate research in marketing. European Journal of Marketing, 32 (9/10), 785–802.
Pinter, H. (1982). A kind of alaska: A premier . Retrieved from http://www.haroldpinter.org/plays/plays_alaska.shtml .
Probst, B. (2015). The eye regards itself: Benefits and challenges of reflexivity in qualitative social work research. Social Work Research Social Work Research , 39 (1), 37–48.
Romano, C. (2015). Freud and the Dora case: A promise betrayed . London: Karnac Books.
Ruddin, L. P. (2006). You can generalize stupid! Social scientists, Bent Flyvbjerg, and case study methodology. Qualitative Inquiry, 12 (4), 797–812.
Sacks, O. (1970). The man who mistook his wife for a hat . New York: Touchstone.
Sacks, O. (1995). An anthropologist on mars: Seven paradoxical tales . New York: Alfred A. Knopf.
Sacks, O. (2015). On the move: A life . New York: Alfred A. Knopf.
Safran, J. D. (2009). Clinical and empirical issues: Disagreements and agreements. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 57 (5), 1043–1069.
Sampson, R. J. (2010). Gold standard myths: Observations on the experimental turn in quantitative criminology. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 26 (4), 489–500.
Sayer, A. (2011). Why things matter to people: Social science, values and ethical life . Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Shaw, E. (2013). Sacred cows and sleeping dogs: Confidentiality: Not as straightforward as we would have thought. Psychotherapy in Australia, 19 (4), 65–66.
Sieck, B. C. (2012). Obtaining clinical writing informed consent versus using client disguise and recommendations for practice. Psychotherapy, 49 (1), 3–11.
Siggelkow, N. (2007). Persuasion with case studies. Academy of Management Journal, 50 (1), 20–24.
Skocpol, T., & Sommers, M. (1980). The uses of comparative theory in macrosocial inquiry. Comparative Studies in Society and History, 22 (2), 174–197.
Smith, C. (2011). What is a person?: Rethinking humanity, social life, and the moral good from the personup . Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Sperry, L., & Pies, R. (2010). Writing about clients: Ethical considerations and options. Counseling and Values, 54 (2), 88–102.
Steinmetz, G. (2004). Odious comparisons: Incommensurability, the case study, and “small N’s” in sociology. Sociological Theory, 22 (3), 371–400.
Steinmetz, G. (2005). The politics of method in the human. In G. Steinmetz (Ed.), Sciences . Durham, NC: Duke University Press.
Sutherland, E. (2016). The case study in telecommunications policy research. Info, 18 (1), 16–30.
Taylor, C., & White, S. (2001). Knowledge, truth and reflexivity: The problem of judgement in social work. Journal of social work , 1 (1), 37–59.
Taylor, C., & White, S. (2006). Knowledge and reasoning in social work: Educating for humane judgement. British Journal of Social Work , 36 (6), 937–954.
Thacher, D. (2006). The Normative Case Study 1. American journal of sociology , 111 (6), 1631–1676.
Tice, K. W. (1998). Tales of wayward girls and immoral women: Case records and the professionalization of social work . Chicago: University of Illinois Press.
Tsang, E. W. (2014). Case studies and generalization in information systems research: A critical realist perspective. The Journal of Strategic Information Systems, 23 (2), 174–186.
Tsang, E. W. (2014). Generalizing from research findings: The merits of case studies. International Journal of Management Reviews, 16 (4), 369–383.
Tufekci, Z. (2015). Algorithmic harms beyond Facebook and Google: Emergent challenges of computational agency. Journal on Telecommunication, 13 , 203–218.
Van de Ven, A. H. (2007). Engaged scholarship: A guide for organizational and social research: A guide for organizational and social research . Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Van Haselen, R. A. (2015). Towards improving the reporting quality of clinical case reports in complementary medicine: Assessing and illustrating the need for guideline development. Complementary Therapies in Medicine, 23 (2), 141–148.
Welch, C., Piekkari, R., Plakoyiannaki, E., & Paavilainen-Mäntymäki, E. (2011). Theorising from case studies: Towards a pluralist future for international business research. Journal of International Business Studies, 42 (5), 740–762.
Wilgus, J., & Wilgus, B. (2009). Face to face with Phineas Gage. Journal of the History of the Neurosciences, 18 (3), 340–345.
Willemsen, J., Cornelis, S., Geerardyn, F. M., Desmet, M., Meganck, R., Inslegers, R., & Cauwe, J. M. (2015). Theoretical pluralism in psychoanalytic case studies. Frontiers in Psychology, 6 , 1466
Willemsen, J., Della Rosa, E., & Kegerreis, S. (2017). Clinical case studies in psychoanalytic and psychodynamic treatment. Frontiers in Psychology, 8 , 1–7. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00108 .
Winship, G. (2007). The ethics of reflective research in single case study inquiry. Perspectives in Psychiatric Care, 43 (4), 174–182.
Wolpert, L., & Fonagy, P. (2009). There is no place for the psychoanalytic case report in the British Journal of Psychiatry. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 195 , 483–487.
Woolcock, M. (2013). Using case studies to explore the external validity of ‘complex’ development interventions. Evaluation, 19 (3), 229–248.
Wynn, D. Jr., & Williams, C. K. (2012). Principles for conducting critical realist case study research in information systems. Mis Quarterly, 36 (3), 787–810.
Yang, D. D. (2006). Empirical social inquiry and models of causal inference. The New England Journal of Political Science, 2 (1), 51–88.
Download references
Author information
Authors and affiliations.
Rutgers School of Social Work, New Brunswick, USA
Jeffrey Longhofer & Jerry Floersch
DSW Program, Rutgers School of Social Work, New Brunswick, USA
Eric Hartmann
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
Corresponding author
Correspondence to Jeffrey Longhofer .
Rights and permissions
Reprints and permissions
About this article
Longhofer, J., Floersch, J. & Hartmann, E. A Case for the Case Study: How and Why They Matter. Clin Soc Work J 45 , 189–200 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10615-017-0631-8
Download citation
Published : 06 June 2017
Issue Date : September 2017
DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/s10615-017-0631-8
Share this article
Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:
Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.
Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative
- Case study research
- Critical realism
- Psychoanaltyic case study
- Social work clinical research
- Find a journal
- Publish with us
- Track your research
Case Studies in Community Psychology Practice: A Global Lens
(2 reviews)
Geraldine L. Palmer, Adler University
Todd L. Rogers
Judah Viola, National Louis University in Chicago
Maronica Engel
Copyright Year: 2022
Publisher: Rebus Community
Language: English
Formats Available
Conditions of use.
Learn more about reviews.
Reviewed by Kara Riggleman, Assistant Professor of Social Work, Taylor University on 11/28/22
I thought this book was very comprehensive, given the subject. There are a variety of community development approaches and models represented, that would be beneficial in a number of contexts. read more
Comprehensiveness rating: 5 see less
I thought this book was very comprehensive, given the subject. There are a variety of community development approaches and models represented, that would be beneficial in a number of contexts.
Content Accuracy rating: 5
This book seems accurate and error-free.
Relevance/Longevity rating: 5
I think the book represents approaches that are important for students to learn, but in language that will be accessible to them.
Clarity rating: 5
Book is clear and concise. A variety of approaches, offered in a concise manner. The text isn't too academic or out of reach for undergraduates.
Consistency rating: 5
Remarkable consistency, even across varied practice settings.
Modularity rating: 5
This text is divided in such a way that an instructor could easily assign only portions of the reading, various case studies, etc. Prior to reviewing this text, I was concerned about students that have difficulty reading a lot of text on a screen. But the text is divided in such a way that even many pages of reading is manageable.
Organization/Structure/Flow rating: 5
Organized in a clear and logical manner.
Interface rating: 2
This is my lowest ranking for this book. Many of the graphics and images were blurry and hard to distinguish.
Grammatical Errors rating: 5
I didn't see any grammatical errors.
Cultural Relevance rating: 5
The authors write with great cultural humility and sensitivity. Each population discussed was treated with honor and due respect.
Overall, I really enjoyed this reading. I think it would be a great supplemental text and help theoretical concepts come to life, via case studies. Seems a very practical "how to" in the work of community development.
Reviewed by Kimberly LaComba, Assistant Professor of Global Leadership, Saint Mary-of-the-Woods College on 11/21/22
This book is current and provides varied perspectives. read more
This book is current and provides varied perspectives.
Would like to see more non-Western references, however, there are interesting and varied perspectives.
Content is relevant and future editions can be easily built upon the current content.
Clearly written through each case study.
The framework is understandable. I was a little concerned that the section was called global perspectives because I thought that would be included in each section’s content.
Appropriate.
The organization and flow is appropriate.
Interface rating: 5
Appropriate infographics/images that further support the content and engage the learner.
Grammatically sound.
Cultural Relevance rating: 4
Would like to see more non-Western references.
This would be a good supplemental text in a course where case studies can be incorporated and storytelling is practiced through a global perspective.
Table of Contents
- 1. Cultural Development in Underrepresented Communities: Using an Empowerment and Citizen Participation Approach
- 2. Dare2Dialogue: Promoting Inclusion Through Storytelling and Dialogue
- 3. Green Space Programs as a Shared Growth and Communal Process: A Somali Gardener’s Journey in Minnesota
- 4. Better Together: Creating Alternative Settings to Reduce Conflict Among Youth in Lebanon
- 5. Promoting Community-Driven Change in Family and Community Systems to Support Girls’ Holistic Development in Senegal
- 6. Lessons from Conducting an Equity-Focused, Participatory Needs Assessment
- 7. Program Evaluation: A Fundamental Component in Effective Community Practice
- 8. Showing up and Standing with: An Intersectional Approach to a Participatory Evaluation of a Housing First Program
- 9. A Plan for Prevention: Measuring Equity from the Start
- 10. Working with Survivors of Gender-Based Violence
- 11. Journeying Past Hurt: Creating and Sustaining Trauma-Informed Healing Practices With Black Pregnant and Parenting Mothers
Ancillary Material
About the book.
There is no better way to demonstrate the work and impact of community psychologists and allies than by showcasing actual projects conducted in partnership with communities. This textbook displays this work in a dynamic case study format that will ignite students' desire and passion to study and become future community psychologists or those whose heart beats with the beloved community. You can find community psychologists and allies partnering with communities to change racist policies, end health disparities, create alternate settings for youth, foster community-based models to heal trauma, evaluate programs, and much more!
About the Contributors
Geraldine (Geri) L. Palmer , Ph.D. (Community Psychology)
Dr. Palmer is the Co-founder/Managing Director of Community Wellness Institute (CWI) in Evanston, IL and an Assistant Professor in the Psychology Department, Clinical Psychology Program at Adler University, Chicago. Dr. Palmer has extensive experience serving in middle and senior executive leadership in the nonprofit/human services sector specifically around the social issues of housing and homelessness. She earned her Ph.D. in Community Psychology from National Louis University where she also teaches a human services management course. Dr. Palmer is an active presenter at academic conferences and workshops, and along with a team of consultants also leads and facilitates the F.A.C. E. of Justice Workshops for CWI. She is a co-author on the chapter, Oppression and Power in Jason, Glantsman, O’Brien and Ramian (Eds) (2019) Introduction to community psychology: An agent of change and author of a chapter, Navigating the Road to Higher Education in Viola and Glantsman (2017) Diverse careers in community psychology. Her writing has been published in a number of peer-reviewed journals. She is currently the Interim Co-Chair of the Council on Cultural, Ethnics and Racial Affairs (CERA), of the Society for Community Research and Action (SCRA).
Todd L. Rogers , M.A. (Industrial/Organization Psychology)
Todd is the Co-founder and Operations Director at Community Wellness Institute. He is an Organizational Psychologist with 20 + years in field operations in the pharmaceutical sector. He is currently working in the facilitation, learning and development and performance analytics space in addition to building a private Narrative Coaching practice. Todd served as a research assistant on CWI’s first research initiative, Exploring Historical Trauma Among Black/African Americans (in progress for publication).
Judah Viola , Ph.D. (Community Psychology)
Judah Viola, PhD is a Community Psychologist and Associate Professor of Psychology at National Louis University in Chicago, Illinois where he co-developed NLU’s PhD program in Community Psychology and previously served as Dean of the College of Professional Studies and Advancement. Judah’s recent publications include Community Psychologists: Who We Are. In L.A. Jason & O. Glantsman, J.F. O’Brien, & K.N. Ramian (Eds.) Introduction to Community Psychology: Becoming an agent of change. Rebus Pressbooks and Diverse Careers in Community Psychology. Oxford University Press. Judah currently serves as the Publications Committee chair for the Society for Community Research and Action. He also manages an independent consulting practice specializing in program evaluation, needs assessment, community building, and collaborative community research. Recent clients have included national and international nonprofits, public school systems, museum and art institutions, social service agencies, and community development organizations. His community research and advocacy interests involve neighborhood revitalization, education for students with disabilities, affordable housing, access to healthcare, access to healthy food, community-police relations, and violence prevention and intervention. For more information see: https://works.bepress.com/judah_viola/
Maronica Engel (Senior Instructional Designer)
Maronica is a senior instructional designer and learning strategist. She has many years of experience in consulting, designing, and developing learning solutions for adult learners in Learning and Development business units in a variety of industries. Maronica assisted with reviewing and editing, and ensuring that the content and layout of the case studies are interesting and engaging, incorporating elements such as photos, links, and visual illustrations. She chose to work on this project because of her strong beliefs in the importance of providing the resources for individuals to be participating and productive agents in their communities; and her passion for education as a life-long learner.
Contribute to this Page
Social Psychology
Index of social psychology pages.
- Chinese Terracotta Warriors
- Otzi the Iceman
- The Antikythera Mechanism
- More Archaeology pages ...
- Stanley Milgram : Obedience to Authority Experiments
- Conformity under Social Pressure : Solomon Asch
- The Nature vs Nurture Debate
- More Social Psychology pages ...
- Stephen Fry quotations and quotes on God and Religion
- Stephen Fry's controversial interview on Irish TV
- Stephen Hawking : God & Religion quotes
- 'God is dead' - Nietzsche
- More Quotations & Quotes pages ...
- Maximilien Robespierre : Reign of Terror
- Otto von Bismarck
- More Historical Biography pages ...
- The European Revolutions of 1848
- Cavour & Italian Unification
- Bismarck & German Unification
- More History pages ...
- The Faith vs. Reason Debate
- World Religions Populations Statistics
- Central spiritual insights
- Other spiritual wisdoms
- More Spirituality pages ...
- Buddha's teachings
- Buddhist Philosophy
- Buddhism vs. Christianity
- More Buddhism pages ...
- Our Downloads page ...
IMAGES
COMMENTS
Jan 1, 2023 · Each of the 10 brilliant social psychology experiments below tells a unique, insightful story relevant to all our lives, every day. Click the link in each social psychology experiment to get the full description and explanation of each phenomenon. 1. Social Psychology Experiments: The Halo Effect.
Sep 1, 2012 · Case-based research is crucial for engaged and relevant community and applied social psychologies, but is often subject to critique. This editorial responds to such critiques, clarifies...
Dec 13, 2023 · Psychology has seen thousands upon thousands of research studies over the years. Most of these studies have helped shape our current understanding of human thoughts, behavior, and feelings.
Twelve sessions of Cognitive Behavioural Therapy (CBT) were ofered. The CBT longitudinal formulation facilitated joint conceptualization of Penny’s early experiences, the negative core beliefs derived and how they contributed to her social anxiety and low mood.
Feb 20, 2024 · Case studies play a crucial role in the field of psychology, providing researchers with valuable insights into human behavior and mental processes. From providing real-life examples to generating hypotheses and analyzing rare phenomena, case studies offer a deep dive into the complexities of the human mind.
Mar 21, 2018 · In Case Studies in Social Psychology: Critical Thinking and Application, Thomas Heinzen and Wind Goodfriend use brief, entertaining case stories to illustrate the historical context...
Jun 6, 2017 · In this special issue we have asked the contributors to make a case for the case study. The guest editors, Jeffrey Longhofer, Jerry Floersch and Eric Hartmann, intergrate ideas from across the disciplines to explore the complexties of case study methods and theory.
This textbook displays this work in a dynamic case study format that will ignite students' desire and passion to study and become future community psychologists or those whose heart beats with the beloved community.
Aug 9, 2012 · The Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology publishes papers that cover the full spectrum of community and social psychology.
The results of Social Psychology research and experiments arranged by Henri Tajfel, Muzafer Sherif, Solomon Asch and others have proven that people, alongside many behaviors we would all, no doubt, applaud, also tend to exhibit some rather perplexing tendencies in group situations!!!