- Research Topics
- New Research
- Oldies but Goodies
- The Critical Eye
- Publications
- CEP Founders
- Faculty & Directors
- Graduate Students
- Online resources
- Suggested reading
- Places to Study EP
- Courses at UCSB
- Research Assistants
- About the CEP
- Public essays
- Press attention
Research topics at the Center for Evolutionary Psychology
Reprints are provided for scholarly purposes only. Permission to reprint any article must be sought from the holder of the copyright.
Scientists at the Center for Evolutionary Psychology specialize in finding new ways that an evolutionary perspective can inform research on the design of the human mind. In so doing, we have been researching many new topics, as well as trying out new approaches to old topics. Below we provide a partial list with links to some relevant research papers.
Principles of evolutionary psychology
A roadmap to principles of evolutionary psychology
Reasoning and rationality
Social exchange reasoning (reciprocation, reciprocal altruism, cooperation)
Judgment under uncertainty (intuitive statistics; optimal foraging; ecological rationality; heuristics & biases)
Adaptationism, normative theories, and rationality
Emotions—What are they?
Emotion (emotions as super-ordinate programs solving the problem of mechanism coordination in a multimodular mind)
Emotions—Specific ones
Anger . See also:
Formidability, Strength, and Entitlement . Adaptations for detecting physical strength
Vision and Visual Attention
Visual attention –a system specialized for monitoring animals
Vision : Faces—the case for domain-specific object recognition
Evolutionary Biology
Evolutionary Biology : Intragenomic Conflict , Pathogens & the Evolution of Sexual Recombination; Banker’s Paradox (etc)
Literature and the Arts
Art, Fiction, and Aesthetics
Close social relationships
Kin detection (as regulator of incest avoidance; altruism)
Friendship and Deep Engagement Relationships
Courtship, Mate Choice, and Human Sexuality
Varieties of Cooperation
Two-person cooperation / reciprocation / reciprocal altruism
Evolution of generosity
Cooperating in Groups:
Coalitional psychology and alliance detection
Adaptations for collective action
Memory (memory systems; specializations; personality trait database; self-knowledge, episodic memories; amnesia; memory loss )
Spatial cognition
Spatial adaptations for foraging (female advantage in location memory for plants; content effects; optimal foraging)
Tools: Cognitive foundations
Adaptations for tool use (the artifact concept and inferences about function; design stance; problem solving, dissociation between inferences about function and naming)
Personality
Personality (personality differences and universal human nature; adaptationist framework for personality science; cognitive systems specialized for encoding, storing, and retrieving knowledge of personality traits)
The links below are not live yet—hopefully I will be able to update them soon! LC
Darwinian medicine, Darwinian psychiatry
Development
Economics, Business, and Organizational Behavior
Hazard Management (precautionary reasoning)
Intelligence (improvisational intelligence; dedicated intelligence; decoupled reasoning (counterfactual reasoning, suppositional reasoning, metarepresentation)
Theoretical foundations of psychology and the behavioral sciences
Environments of Evolutionary Adaptedness (EEA; Why the past explains the present)
Evolutionary psychology: A primer
A brief introduction to the field in [ English ], [ Español ] and [ Português ].
Recent News
Recent Awards
Evolutionary Psychology Research Paper Topics
This page provides a comprehensive list of evolutionary psychology research paper topics , designed to guide students in exploring the depths of human behavior through the lens of evolution. Evolutionary psychology melds principles from psychology, biology, and anthropology to understand how evolutionary processes shape cognitive functions and behaviors that were advantageous for survival and reproduction. The selected topics delve into various aspects such as mating strategies, social behavior, and the evolutionary basis of mental health issues, highlighting the field’s potential to explain both contemporary human behavior and psychological traits. By engaging with these topics, students will not only enhance their academic knowledge but also contribute to a broader understanding of human nature, equipped to address complex psychological questions with evolutionary insights. This resource aims to inspire and facilitate rigorous academic inquiry into how evolutionary past influences present psychological outcomes, fostering a richer understanding of the human condition.
100 Evolutionary Psychology Research Paper Topics
Evolutionary psychology is a dynamic field that integrates insights from psychology, biology, anthropology, and cognitive science to explore the origins and functions of human behavior and cognition. This interdisciplinary approach provides a robust framework for understanding how evolutionary processes influence modern human behavior and psychological traits. Choosing the right evolutionary psychology research paper topics is crucial as these topics can illuminate our understanding of human nature and behavior through an evolutionary lens. By investigating these topics, students can contribute to a deeper comprehension of why humans behave, think, and feel the way they do today, based on adaptations that occurred in response to challenges faced by our ancestors.
Academic Writing, Editing, Proofreading, And Problem Solving Services
Get 10% off with 24start discount code.
- The principles of natural selection in psychological contexts
- Evolutionary theories of brain development
- The role of genetics in psychological traits
- Comparative psychology: Insights from animal studies
- The adaptationist approach to human psychology
- Evolutionary game theory and human behavior
- Methods and approaches in evolutionary psychology research
- Historical figures and milestones in evolutionary psychology
- Criticisms and controversies in evolutionary psychology
- The future of evolutionary psychology
- The evolution of human mating behaviors
- Sexual selection and its impact on human psychological traits
- Parental investment theories in human development
- The role of attractiveness in mate selection
- Strategies of intrasexual competition
- Mate choice and preference across cultures
- Evolutionary explanations for infidelity and jealousy
- The impact of social status on mating success
- The biology of human sexual orientation
- Gender differences in sexual psychology
- Theories of parental investment and offspring survival
- Evolutionary perspectives on family structure and cohesion
- The role of kin selection in altruistic behaviors within families
- Parent-child relationships from an evolutionary standpoint
- Conflict and cooperation in sibling interactions
- The impact of parental age on reproductive strategies
- Evolutionary views on adoption and step-parenting
- Cross-cultural variations in family dynamics
- The effects of parental behavior on child development
- The evolution of human parenting skills
- The origins of social cooperation and community living
- Evolutionary bases of group dynamics and leadership
- The development of social hierarchies
- Territorial behaviors and their psychological implications
- The role of language in social evolution
- Trust and reputation in evolutionary perspective
- The evolution of conflict and peacemaking
- Social exclusion and its adaptive significance
- Cooperative breeding and communal care in human history
- The evolution of human empathy and moral reasoning
- The biological basis of altruistic behaviors in humans
- Reciprocal altruism and its conditions
- Costly signaling and altruism as a social strategy
- The role of altruism in modern societies
- Evolutionary models of cooperation and fairness
- The genetic and environmental influences on altruistic behavior
- The survival value of altruism in human evolution
- Psychological and physiological benefits of being altruistic
- Altruism in crisis situations: An evolutionary analysis
- The limits and potential selfishness underlying altruistic acts
- The evolution of human intelligence and cognitive functions
- Decision-making processes from an evolutionary perspective
- The adaptiveness of cognitive biases and heuristics
- Memory systems and their evolutionary origins
- The role of perception in survival and reproduction
- Theory of mind and its evolutionary significance
- The evolutionary underpinnings of creativity and innovation
- Cognitive adaptations in response to environmental challenges
- The interplay between cognition and culture in human evolution
- Evolutionary explanations for cognitive disorders
- The adaptive functions of human emotions
- Evolutionary explanations for emotional expressions
- The role of emotions in survival and social interactions
- Fear, anger, joy, and sadness from an evolutionary viewpoint
- The neurobiology of emotions and its evolutionary aspects
- Emotional intelligence as an evolutionary adaptation
- The impact of emotions on decision-making and behavior
- The evolution of emotional disorders
- Cross-cultural studies on emotional perception and expression
- Attachment and love: Evolutionary perspectives
- The evolutionary basis of personality traits
- Personality and survival: An adaptive approach
- The role of personality in mate selection and reproductive success
- Heritability of personality features and evolutionary psychology
- The Big Five personality traits from an evolutionary angle
- Personality disorders: An evolutionary analysis
- The influence of personality on social hierarchy and group dynamics
- Stability and change in personality across the lifespan
- Environmental influences on the development of personality traits
- Personality and health outcomes from an evolutionary perspective
- The evolutionary roots of criminal behavior
- Psychopathy and evolutionary theories of social behavior
- Evolutionary explanations for aggression and violence
- Sexual coercion and evolutionary insights into criminal acts
- The adaptiveness of deceptive and manipulative behaviors
- Evolutionary perspectives on legal decision-making
- The role of evolutionary psychology in criminal profiling
- Evolutionary theories in understanding victimology
- The implications of evolutionary psychology for penal systems
- Moral and ethical considerations in evolutionary forensic psychology
- The evolution of cultural norms and their psychological impacts
- The role of culture in shaping human evolution
- Cultural transmission and its mechanisms from an evolutionary perspective
- Cooperation and competition in cultural groups
- The co-evolution of language and social structures
- The impact of religion on social cohesion and moral behaviors
- Evolutionary approaches to the study of myths and rituals
- Cultural diversity and adaptive strategies in human societies
- The influence of cultural evolution on human cognition
- Future directions in the study of cultural evolution
The exploration of evolutionary psychology research paper topics offers profound insights into the origins and functions of human behavior. These topics span a diverse range of areas, each contributing to a deeper understanding of how evolutionary principles shape our actions, thoughts, and feelings. Students are encouraged to delve into these evolutionary psychology research paper topics, as they hold the potential to not only enrich academic knowledge but also provide practical applications that can inform areas such as therapy, education, policy, and interpersonal relationships. Engaging with these topics is an opportunity to advance our collective understanding of human nature in the context of its evolutionary past.
The Range of Evolutionary Psychology Research Paper Topics
Adaptive Functions of Psychological Traits
The core of evolutionary psychology lies in its attempt to understand psychological traits as adaptations designed to solve recurrent problems faced by our ancestors. This perspective suggests that many aspects of human behavior, including our emotional responses and cognitive abilities, have developed to increase our ancestors’ survival and reproductive success. For instance, fear responses and threat detection mechanisms are crucial adaptive traits that evolved to help early humans avoid predators and other environmental hazards, enhancing their chances of survival and reproduction.
Moreover, reproductive and mating behaviors are central to evolutionary psychology. The field examines various behaviors from mate selection to parental investment, providing a biological and psychological basis for understanding complex social behaviors. For example, evolutionary psychology explores how preferences for certain traits in mates may have evolved to maximize reproductive success and ensure the survival of offspring, highlighting the intricate link between psychological predispositions and reproductive strategies.
Evolutionary Approaches to Mental Health
Evolutionary psychology research paper topics often address the origins of mental health issues by considering them as potential evolutionary mismatches—conditions where the modern environment differs significantly from the ancestral environments in which our psychological traits evolved. This framework can shed light on the prevalence of depression and anxiety in modern societies, suggesting that these disorders may be linked to living conditions that are vastly different from those our evolutionary ancestors faced. For instance, the high rates of depression in highly urbanized and socially fragmented environments can be interpreted through this lens, suggesting a mismatch between our social and environmental needs and what contemporary life offers.
In addition to explaining the origins of mental disorders, evolutionary psychology also considers how our evolutionary past might inform therapeutic approaches. Understanding that certain aspects of modern life may clash with our evolved dispositions can guide the development of treatment plans that better align with our inherent tendencies, potentially improving therapeutic outcomes for a range of psychological disorders.
Cultural Variations and Evolutionary Psychology
Exploring evolutionary psychology research paper topics extends into understanding cultural differences in behaviors and mental processes. This area of study examines how evolutionary pressures may have varied across different environments, leading to cultural diversifications in psychological traits. For instance, risk-taking behaviors might be more prevalent in cultures that historically faced more unpredictable environments, where taking risks could lead to greater rewards.
The interaction between genetic predispositions and cultural expressions also illustrates the adaptive nature of human psychology. Genes provide a framework within which psychological traits develop, but cultural contexts can significantly modulate how these traits are expressed. Additionally, the role of cultural evolution in shaping psychological phenomena suggests that cultural practices themselves may evolve to enhance the fit between human psychology and its environment, further contributing to the diversity of human behavior observed across the globe.
The insights gained from evolutionary psychology research paper topics are invaluable for deepening our understanding of human psychology. By framing psychological traits as products of evolutionary processes, this field provides a comprehensive perspective that integrates biology, psychology, and anthropology. The potential of evolutionary psychology research topics to revolutionize our understanding of human nature is immense, offering ways to enhance psychological interventions and improve mental health outcomes by aligning them more closely with our evolutionary heritage. This approach not only enriches academic knowledge but also has practical implications for developing more effective mental health strategies and fostering healthier societies.
iResearchNet’s Writing Services
For evolutionary psychology research papers.
At iResearchNet, we specialize in providing exceptional writing services tailored specifically for the nuanced needs of evolutionary psychology research. Our services are designed to support students and researchers as they delve into the complex intersections of psychology, biology, and anthropology to explore how evolutionary past shapes current human behavior. Whether you are investigating mating strategies, social behaviors, or the evolutionary bases of mental disorders, iResearchNet equips you with the resources and expert guidance necessary to produce insightful, well-researched papers that contribute meaningfully to the field of evolutionary psychology.
- Expert degree-holding writers : Our team is composed of writers who not only possess advanced degrees in psychology but have specialized knowledge in evolutionary psychology, ensuring that your research paper is informed by expert insights and deep understanding of the field.
- Custom written works : Every paper is crafted from scratch, tailored to meet your specific academic requirements and research interests, ensuring a unique and customized research output.
- In-depth research : We commit to comprehensive research, utilizing up-to-date, credible sources to enrich your paper with the most relevant and current information in the field of evolutionary psychology.
- Custom formatting (APA, MLA, Chicago/Turabian, Harvard) : Our writers are proficient in all academic formatting styles, ensuring that your paper meets your institution’s specific formatting requirements.
- Top quality : We uphold the highest standards of quality in every paper we deliver, ensuring that each piece is well-organized, thoroughly edited, and polished to meet academic excellence.
- Customized solutions : We understand that evolutionary psychology encompasses a broad range of topics. Our services are highly flexible, designed to cater to the particular needs of each research topic.
- Flexible pricing : Our pricing model is designed to be competitive and accommodating, ensuring that students can access high-quality writing services without financial strain.
- Short deadlines up to 3 hours : For those urgent deadlines, we offer a rapid turnaround without compromising the quality or depth of research, helping you meet your academic deadlines on time.
- Timely delivery : We guarantee the on-time delivery of all papers, respecting your submission timelines and helping you stay on track with your academic schedule.
- 24/7 support : Our support team is available around the clock to provide you with assistance, answer any queries you might have, and support you throughout the writing process.
- Absolute privacy : We adhere to strict privacy policies to ensure that all your personal and academic information remains confidential and secure.
- Easy order tracking : Our user-friendly online platform allows for seamless order tracking, giving you peace of mind and keeping you updated on the progress of your research paper.
- Money-back guarantee : We stand behind the quality of our work with a money-back guarantee, offering you assurance and confidence in our writing services.
At iResearchNet, we are deeply committed to supporting the academic journey of students in evolutionary psychology with our specialized writing services. By choosing us, you gain access to a wealth of knowledge and a team of professionals dedicated to helping you succeed. We are proud to offer high-quality, customized solutions that not only meet but exceed your academic expectations. Trust us to be your partner in navigating the fascinating field of evolutionary psychology, and let us help you make a significant impact through your research.
Buy Your Custom Research Paper Today!
Unlock the full potential of your academic research with iResearchNet, your premier destination for expert writing services in evolutionary psychology. We understand the challenges and complexities that come with exploring the depths of human behavior from an evolutionary perspective. That’s why we are here to support you every step of the way, from initial research to the final draft of your research paper.
Take advantage of our specialized services to navigate the intriguing field of evolutionary psychology with confidence. Our team of expert degree-holding writers is well-versed in the nuances of the discipline and committed to providing research papers that are not only comprehensive but also rich in scholarly insight. Whether you’re exploring mating behaviors, social dynamics, or the evolutionary underpinnings of psychological disorders, iResearchNet is equipped to help you develop a paper that meets rigorous academic standards.
We make the process of ordering your custom research paper as easy as possible. Our user-friendly online platform allows you to place your order in just a few clicks. Specify your requirements, set your deadlines, and leave the rest to us. Our flexible pricing plans are designed to fit your budget, and our short deadline options ensure that even the most urgent assignments are handled with precision and care.
Plus, with our 24/7 customer support, you have continuous access to assistance whenever you need it. Track the progress of your order at any stage and benefit from our absolute privacy policy that guarantees the security of your personal and project information. And with our money-back guarantee, you can be confident in the quality and reliability of our services.
Don’t miss the opportunity to excel in your studies and contribute meaningful research to the field of evolutionary psychology. Buy your custom research paper from iResearchNet today and experience the difference that professional support makes. Empower your academic journey with iResearchNet and achieve the success you deserve in evolutionary psychology.
ORDER HIGH QUALITY CUSTOM PAPER
Loading metrics
Open Access
Essays articulate a specific perspective on a topic of broad interest to scientists.
See all article types »
Darwin in Mind: New Opportunities for Evolutionary Psychology
* E-mail: [email protected] (JJB); [email protected] (KNL)
Affiliation Behavioural Biology Group and Helmholtz Institute, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands
Affiliation School of Psychology, University of St Andrews, St Andrews, Scotland, United Kingdom
Affiliation Department of Philosophy, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, Ohio, United States of America
Affiliation School of Biology, University of St Andrews, St Andrews, Scotland, United Kingdom
- Johan J. Bolhuis,
- Gillian R. Brown,
- Robert C. Richardson,
- Kevin N. Laland
Published: July 19, 2011
- https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1001109
- Reader Comments
Evolutionary Psychology (EP) views the human mind as organized into many modules, each underpinned by psychological adaptations designed to solve problems faced by our Pleistocene ancestors. We argue that the key tenets of the established EP paradigm require modification in the light of recent findings from a number of disciplines, including human genetics, evolutionary biology, cognitive neuroscience, developmental psychology, and paleoecology. For instance, many human genes have been subject to recent selective sweeps; humans play an active, constructive role in co-directing their own development and evolution; and experimental evidence often favours a general process, rather than a modular account, of cognition. A redefined EP could use the theoretical insights of modern evolutionary biology as a rich source of hypotheses concerning the human mind, and could exploit novel methods from a variety of adjacent research fields.
Citation: Bolhuis JJ, Brown GR, Richardson RC, Laland KN (2011) Darwin in Mind: New Opportunities for Evolutionary Psychology. PLoS Biol 9(7): e1001109. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1001109
Copyright: © 2011 Bolhuis et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Funding: JJB is funded by Utrecht University and by Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO) grants (ALW Open Competition and GW Horizon Programme) ( http://www.nwo.nl/ ). GRB is funded by a Wellcome Trust Career Development Fellowship, UK ( http://www.wellcome.ac.uk/ ). RCR is funded by the University of Cincinnati. KNL is funded by the BBSRC, UK ( http://www.bbsrc.ac.uk/ ) and an ERC Advanced Grant http://erc.europa.eu/ ). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Abbreviations: AI, artificial intelligence; EEA, environment of evolutionary adaptedness; EP, Evolutionary Psychology
In the century and a half since Charles Darwin's publication of the Origin of Species , evolutionary theory has become the bedrock of modern biology; yet, its application to the human mind remains steeped in controversy [1] – [13] . Darwin himself wrote of cognitive evolution, most notably in The Descent of Man , where he suggested that like any other trait, human “mental faculties” are the outcome of evolution by natural and sexual selection and insisted that they should be understood in light of what he called “common descent”. This evolutionary interpretation of human cognition was taken up in the 1980s by contemporary evolutionary psychology, which rapidly became dominated by a school of thought stemming from the University of California at Santa Barbara (see Box 1 ). The essence of this brand of Evolutionary Psychology (EP) is neatly summarized in the famous quote that “Our modern skulls house a Stone Age mind” [2] .
Box 1. The Major Tenets of Evolutionary Psychology
According to the Santa Barbara school of Evolutionary Psychology (EP), human minds are organized into a large number of evolved psychological mechanisms—psychological adaptations designed to solve recurrent problems faced by our hunter-gatherer ancestors [30] . These evolutionary psychologists attempt to provide criteria for “carving the mind at its natural joints” [104] , generally by reverse-engineering from an observable phenomenon to its proposed function.
In the 1980s, four major tenets of EP crystallized, and these ideas became widespread. While not all evolutionary psychologists endorse the Santa Barbara perspective, these ideas have nonetheless shaped the broader field, and remain extremely prevalent.
1. The environment of evolutionary adaptedness ( EEA ). This concept refers to the notion that our psychological mechanisms have evolved in response to stable features of ancestral environments [87] . While the EEA has frequently been equated with an African Pleistocene savanna, this version of the concept has been strongly critiqued [66] , and the more recent formulation of the EEA concept presents a broader, less specific theoretical landscape of our past lives, based on an abstract statistical composite of all relevant past selective environments [105] .
2. Gradualism. Evolutionary psychologists argue that minds are built from co-adapted gene complexes that are unable to respond quickly to selection [105] , [106] . When combined with the concept of the EEA, gradualism suggests that human beings experience an adaptive lag [88] , such that evolved psychological mechanisms may not produce adaptive responses in modern human environments that have undergone dramatic recent changes [105] .
3. Massive modularity. Given that different sets of adaptive problems will have required different computational solutions, the mind is argued to consist predominantly of domain-specific, modular programmes [105] . Whether the mind also contains evolved general-purpose processes remains debated within EP [104] .
4. Universal human nature. The evolved computational programmes in the human mind are assumed to be responsible for producing a universal (that is, species-typical) human nature [105] . At the same time, different outcomes of these programmes are suggested to be triggered by different environmental or social conditions, leading to the prediction of both universal behavioural outcomes and locally specified adaptive solutions [105] .
However, many evolutionarily minded psychologists, evolutionary biologists, and philosophers of science disagree with the theoretical proposals put forward by the Santa Barbara evolutionary psychologists, and the discipline has been the subject of intense debates [1] , [3] – [13] . Here, we assess the impact of recent developments in genetics, evolutionary and developmental biology, paleoecology, and cognitive science on EP and then go on to suggest that these developments provide new avenues for research.
Reassessing the Major Tenets of Evolutionary Psychology
EP is encapsulated by four major tenets (see Box 1 ) that have generated considerable discussion. Here, we argue that all of these basic assumptions need to be reassessed in the light of contemporary evidence.
The Environment of Evolutionary Adaptedness and Gradualism
EP argues that that human cognitive processes evolved in response to selection pressures acting in ancestral conditions—in an environment of evolutionary adaptedness (EEA)—and are not necessarily adaptive in a contemporary world that has changed radically in recent millennia. From this vantage point, genetic evolution simply could not keep pace fully with the extraordinary rate at which human technology transformed environments. Tied up with this notion of adaptive lag (or mismatch between our biology and our environment) is an emphasis on evolutionary gradualism: evolutionary change, particularly with respect to complex adaptations in the human mind, is deemed to have occurred slowly; too slowly to have led to significant genetic change in the few hundred generations that have elapsed since the end of the Pleistocene, or even since the spread of modern humans around the world over the last 50,000 years.
Recent developments in human genetics have challenged the concepts of adaptive lag and gradualism. EP originated in the early 1980s, when our knowledge of the human genome was limited and gradualism dominated evolutionary thinking (although biologists' attempts to estimate rates of selection in nature were in full flow in the 1970s [14] , leaving the Santa Barbara school's gradualism assumption contentious from the outset). Since then, geneticists have not only mapped the genome, but have devised means for detecting which genes have been subject to recent selection [15] – [19] . There have been substantial human genetic changes in the last 50,000 years, with possibly as much as 10% of human genes affected [19] . Events in the Holocene (the last 10,000 years), particularly the adoption of agriculture, domestication of animals, and the increases in human densities that these practices afforded, were a major source of selection on our species [17] – [22] , and possibly accelerated human evolution [20] , [22] . Evidence from the human genome strongly suggests that recent human evolution has been affected by responses to features of the environment that were constructed by humans, from culturally facilitated changes in diet, to aspects of modern living that inadvertently promoted the spread of diseases [22] , [23] . Genes expressed in the human brain are well-represented in this recent selection [11] , [12] .
Evolutionary biologists have also measured the rate of response to selection in a wide variety of animals [14] , [24] , finding that evolutionary change typically occurs much faster than hitherto thought. A recent meta-analysis of 63 studies that measured the strength of natural selection in 62 species, including more than 2,500 estimates of selection, concluded that the median selection gradient (a measure of the rate of change of fitness with trait value) was 0.16, which would cause a quantitative trait to change by one standard deviation in just 25 generations [24] . If humans exhibit equivalent rates, then significant genetic evolution would occur over the course of a few hundred years. While fast evolution is far from inevitable, there is nonetheless strong evidence that it has frequently occurred in humans. EP has yet to come to terms with the possibility of recent, rapid genetic changes with their potential for associated neural rewiring.
Even if we consider the selection pressures that acted on ancestral human populations during the Pleistocene epoch (approximately 1.7 million to 10,000 years ago), the abstract concept of stable selection pressures in the EEA is challenged by recent evidence from paleoecology and paleoanthropology. The Pleistocene was apparently far from stable, not only being variable, but progressively changing in the pattern of variation [25] , [26] . The world experienced by members of the genus Homo in the early Pleistocene was very different from that experienced in the late Pleistocene, and even early anatomical modern Homo sapiens that lived around 150,000 years ago led very different lives from Upper Paleolithic people (40,000 years ago) [27] – [29] .
Universalism
EP has also placed emphasis on the concept of human nature, comprising a species-specific repertoire of universal, evolved psychological mechanisms, from a childhood fear of strangers, to a cheater-detection mechanism, to a preference for specific mate characteristics. This putative universal cognition can be rendered compatible with the observed diversity in human behaviour by recourse to context-dependent strategies. From this perspective, the mind shifts between pre-specified behavioural outputs in response to differential environmental influences [30] , [31] .
This explanation of human behavioral variation is also contentious [3] , [32] – [34] . The notion of universalism has led to the view that undergraduates at Western universities constitute a representative sample of human nature, a view that has been subject to criticism from anthropologists and psychologists [33] – [35] . Moreover, by EP's formulation, all epigenetic and developmental effects simply evoke alternative genetically pre-specified strategies. Recent trends in developmental psychology and neuroscience have instead stressed the malleability of the human brain, emphasizing how experience tunes and regulates synaptic connectivity, neural circuitry and gene expression in the brain, leading to remarkable plasticity in the brain's structural and functional organization [36] . Neuroscientists have been aware since the 1980s that the human brain has too much architectural complexity for it to be plausible that genes specify its wiring in detail [37] ; therefore, developmental processes carry much of the burden of establishing neural connections.
In parallel, emerging trends in evolutionary theory, particularly the growth of developmental systems theory, epigenetic inheritance, and niche-construction theory, have placed emphasis on organisms as active constructors of their environments [38] – [40] . The development of an organism, including the characteristics of its brain, involves a complex interaction between genetically inherited information, epigenetic influences, and learning in response to constructed features of the physical and social environment [5] , [40] – [45] . From this viewpoint, the human mind does not consist of pre-specified programmes, but is built via a constant interplay between the individual and its environment [45] , [46] , a point made by developmental psychologist Daniel Lehrman [47] many years ago. By constructing their worlds (for example, by building homes, planting crops, and setting up social institutions), humans co-direct their own development and evolution [22] , [39] , [48] , [49] .
The view that a universal genetic programme underpins human cognition is also not fully consistent with current genetic evidence. Humans are less genetically diverse than many species, including other apes [50] , largely because human effective population sizes were small until around 70,000 years ago [51] , [52] . Nonetheless, there is enough genetic variation to have supported considerable adaptive change in the intervening time, and recent thinking amongst geneticists is that our species' unique reliance on learned behaviour and culture may have relaxed allowable thresholds for large-scale genomic diversity [21] , [53] . Human behavioral genetics has also identified genetic variation underlying an extensive list of cognitive and behavioural characteristics [54] .
While variation within populations accounts for the bulk of human genetic variation, around 5%–7% of genetic differences can be attributed to variation between populations [55] . Some of the significant genetic differences between human populations have arisen from recent selective events [56] , [57] . Gene-culture coevolution may well turn out to be the characteristic pattern of evolutionary change in humans over recent time spans [22] , [58] (see Box 2 ). From this perspective, cultural practices are likely to have influenced selection pressures on the human brain, raising the possibility that genetic variation could lead to biases in the human cognitive processing between, as well as within, populations. In summary, there is no uniform human genetic program.
Box 2. Gene-Culture Coevolution
Gene-culture coevolutionary theory explores how genetic and cultural processes interact over evolutionary time [22] , [58] . Changes in diet afforded by cultural practices, such as agriculture and the domestication of plants and animals, provide compelling examples of gene-culture coevolution, demonstrating how cultural practices have transformed the selection pressures acting on humans and given rise to some of the genetic differences between human populations. For instance, there is now little doubt that dairy farming created the selective environment that favoured the spread of alleles for adult lactose tolerance [85] , [107] , [108] . Another example concerns the evolution of the human amylase gene: Perry et al. [109] found that copy number of the salivary amylase gene ( AMY1 ) is positively correlated with salivary amylase protein level and that individuals from human populations with high-starch diets have, on average, more AMY1 copies than those with traditionally low-starch diets. Indeed, the transition to novel food sources with the advent of agriculture and the colonization of new habitats would appear to have been a major source of selection on humans [17] , [110] , and several genes related to the metabolism of carbohydrates, lipids, and phosphates show signals of recent selection [17] – [19] .
More generally, human dispersal and subsequent exposure to novel climates, aggregation and exposure to new pathogens, and farming and exposure to new diets are now widely thought to be the source of selection for the spread of many human alleles [22] . Amongst the overrepresented categories in genome-wide scans of recent selection are numerous alleles expressed in the human nervous system and brain [17] – [19] . This raises the possibility that complex cognition on which culture is reliant (social intelligence, language, and challenges associated with constructing and adapting to new environmental conditions) have driven human brain evolution. Mathematical models exploring how genetic and cultural processes interact provide strong support for the role of gene-culture coevolution in human evolution [92] , [111] – [115] . Analyses of these models has often revealed patterns and rates of change that are uncharacteristic of more traditional population genetic theory [92,114–116]. Gene-culture dynamics are typically faster and stronger and operate over a broader range of conditions than conventional evolutionary dynamics [22] , [83] , [117] , [118] .
EP's emphasis on a universal human nature has hindered its exploitation of new opportunities to examine human diversity utilizing evolutionary biology. Contemporary evolution theory makes predictions about behavioural variation within and between populations in traits commonly studied by evolutionary psychologists. For example, sex differences in mate preferences constitute a large proportion of EP research and are generally assumed to exhibit universal patterns (e.g., [59] , [60] ); however, sexual selection theory suggests that a number of factors, such as sex-biased mortality, population density, and variation in mate quality, will affect sex roles (see Box 3 ). A modern EP would make greater use of the theoretical insights of modern evolutionary biology as a source of testable hypotheses [3] , [6] .
Box 3. Reconsidering the Evolution of Sex Roles
Based on the classic work of Bateman [119] and Trivers [120] , EP has predicted sex differences in the relative competiveness and choosiness of men and women when seeking mating partners. Men are generally assumed to have been selected to favour more sexual partners than women and to base their choices on the age, health, and physical attractiveness of prospective partners; in contrast, women are assumed to be more choosy than men and to base their judgements on the willingness of males to invest resources in their offspring [59] . However, contemporary sexual selection theory [121] , [122] suggests that a number of factors, such as sex-biased mortality, population density, and variation in mate quality, will affect how competitive and choosy males and females are, with sex roles expected to vary considerably within and between societies. For example, this theory predicts that, in human beings, both sexes will be choosy when encounter rates with potential mates are high, particularly where the parental investment levels of both sexes are large and not too different, and/or where variation in mate quality of both sexes is high, and males are likely to be choosy in populations with a female-biased adult sex ratio and considerable paternal investment.
The prediction that sex roles will vary between populations is borne out in data on variance in mating and reproductive success in current and historic human populations, which does not support the notion of a single universal pattern [123] . In addition, evolutionary psychologists have themselves begun to record cross-cultural variation in mate preferences and to examine whether variables such as adult sex ratios and local pathogen loads can explain within- and between-population variation in mating behaviour (e.g., [31] ). However, the EP perspective generally assumes that context-specific strategies are pre-programmed within our evolved psychological mechanisms, such that individuals possess multiple strategies that are differentially elicited by certain external factors or that individuals develop a particular strategy as a result of environmental inputs acting on evolved developmental systems during early life (e.g., [30] , [60] . Arguably, the more flexible and variable the exhibited behaviour, the less explanatory power can be attributed to evolved structure in the mind.
An alternative perspective, supported by developmental systems and niche construction theorists (e.g., [38] , [39] ), posits that the human mind does not consist solely of pre-specified programmes and that brain development is strongly influenced by transmitted culture. One of the key contrasts between this perspective and traditional EP is therefore the role that socially transmitted culture has to play in the development of the brain and behaviour [32] . For illustration, consider how the relatively recent developments of agriculture (niche construction), high-density populations, and the evolution of social stratification (transmitted culture), have dramatically changed the ecological context of human mating decisions from what would have occurred in hunter-gatherer societies. According to the aforementioned theory, the increasing encounter rates that such practices likely afforded should have led to much greater choosiness in both modern men and women compared to their Pleistocene ancestors. Modern evolutionary theory has much to offer evolutionary psychologists who are willing to eschew a focus on universality.
Massive Modularity
EP has proposed that the mind consists of evolved cognitive modules, a perspective referred to as the massive modularity hypothesis [61] , [62] . Massive modularity is a somewhat idiosyncratic interpretation of Fodor's [63] original concept of modularity. Essentially, Fodor suggested that what he called input systems (such as those involved in auditory and visual perception, but also in language) were modular, i.e., operating in relative isolation from each other. Information from these modular systems would be passed on to central systems (involved in problem solving or thought) that themselves were thought not to be modular. EP has extended modularity to involve the whole mind/brain.
The thesis of massive modularity is not supported by the neuroscientific evidence [64] – [67] . Firstly, comparative psychology presents an unassailable case for the existence of domain-general mechanisms. The processes of associative learning are widespread in animals and have general properties that allow animals to learn about the causal relationships among a wide variety of events [68] , [69] . For instance, a simple learning theory rule, known as the Rescorla–Wagner rule [70] , has proved extraordinarily useful in explaining the results of hundreds of experiments in diverse animals, including foraging in honeybees, avoidance conditioning in goldfish, and inferential reasoning in humans.
Secondly, there is broad involvement of diverse neural structures in many psychological processes, and there is feedback even to the most basic perceptual processing. For instance, the hominid brain has not only witnessed a proportional expansion of the neocortex, but the neocortex has become intricately interconnected and has evolved projections into the medulla and spinal cord [71] . This has allowed humans to learn intricate routines of movement and complex manual tasks, because the Fodorian executive part of the brain can directly monitor the fingers and the feet [71] . The same projections allow exhibit fine control of the tongue, vocal chords, and breathing, without which humans probably could not have learned to speak [71] . After evaluating the evidence and consistent with Fodor's original proposals, Bolhuis and Macphail [64] suggested that there is no evidence for modularity in central systems such as those involved in learning and memory. With regard to cognitive mechanisms, more often than not, data from animal experiments is consistent with a general-process account rather than an interpretation involving adaptively specialized cognitive modules [64] , [65] , [67] , [72] .
A large part of EP's emphasis on massive modularity drew from artificial intelligence (AI) research. While the great lesson from AI research of the 1970s was that domain specificity was critical to intelligent behaviour, the lesson of the new millennium is that intelligent agents (such as driverless robotic cars) require integration and decision-making across domains, regularly utilize general-process tools such as Bayesian analysis, stochastic modelling, and optimization, and are responsive to a variety of environmental cues [73] . However, while AI research has shifted away from an emphasis on domain specificity, some evolutionary psychologists continue to argue that selection would have favoured predominantly domain-specific mechanisms (e.g., [74] ). In contrast, others have started to present the case for domain-general evolved psychological mechanisms (e.g., [75] , [76] ), and evidence from developmental psychology suggests that domain-general learning mechanisms frequently build on knowledge acquired through domain-specific perceptual processes and core cognition [44] . Both domain-specific and domain-general mechanisms are compatible with evolutionary theory, and their relative importance in human information processing will only be revealed through careful experimentation, leading to a greater understanding of how the brain works [44] .
Towards a New Science of the Evolution of the Mind
We have reviewed how developments in a number of scientific fields have called into question the key tenets of EP. Fortunately, these developments do not just create problems for EP, but also suggest potential solutions. We argue that the key factor will be the methodological and conceptual integration of EP with adjacent fields.
Traditionally, EP has tested hypotheses using the conventional tools of psychology (questionnaires, computer-based experiments, etc.). Generally these hypotheses have a functional perspective—that is, EP proposes that a particular mechanism functioned to enhance reproductive success in our ancestors. However, Nobel laureate Niko Tinbergen [77] famously proposed that understanding behavior requires comprehension not only of its function and evolution, but also of its causation and development [78] , and he argued that a complete understanding of behavior involves addressing all four of these questions. These distinctions are relevant because accounts of the evolution of brain and cognition cannot in themselves explain the brain's underlying working mechanisms [1] , since these are logically distinct questions. While evolutionary analyses may generate clues as to the mechanisms of human cognition, these are best regarded as hypotheses, not established explanations, that need to be tested empirically [1] , [64] , [79] , and there are instances where such evolutionary hypotheses about mechanisms have had to be rejected [1] . Here, we ask which of Tinbergen's questions is currently addressed in the field of EP and describe how EP could expand its focus to provide a broader and richer understanding of human behaviour.
Evolutionary psychologists commonly seek to study how the human mind works by using knowledge of evolution to formulate, and sometimes test, hypotheses concerning the function of cognitive architecture. While functional or evolutionary considerations cannot be used to test hypotheses about mechanisms, considerations in one domain can generate hypotheses concerning problems in the other domain. For instance, a theory of the evolution of a certain cognitive trait may generate hypotheses as to the mechanisms of that trait. Evolutionary psychologists have conducted hundreds of empirical studies to test the predictions generated by consideration of evolutionary arguments [80] . However, we should be clear that such studies do not test the evolutionary hypotheses themselves, but rather test whether the predictions about the psychological mechanisms have been upheld [6] , [81] . For example, the numerous studies supporting the hypothesis that human beings are predisposed to detect cheaters in social situations [74] , [82] are consistent with several evolutionary explanations. While the original researchers reasoned that cheater detection has resulted from a selective history of reciprocal altruism [82] , alternative evolutionary explanations, for instance that a history of cultural group selection has selected for this trait [83] , and non-evolutionary explanations, are also plausible.
The recent trend within the behavioural sciences has been away from confirmation or rejection of a single hypothesis towards the far more powerful simultaneous evaluation of multiple competing statistical models through model selection procedures [84] . A modern EP would, as standard practice, conduct empirical studies designed specifically to test between multiple competing adaptive and non-adaptive explanations [13] , and would test the evolutionary historical, as well as the proximate, aspects of its hypotheses. In the following sections, we examine how EP could expand to cover all four of Tinbergen's questions.
i) A modern EP would evaluate the evolution of a character by constructing and testing population genetic models, estimating and measuring responses to selection, exploring the covariation of phenotypic traits or genetic variation with putative selective agents, making comparisons across species and seeking correlates to selected traits in the selective environment, and so forth, as do contemporary evolutionary biologists. In addition to these established tools, researchers can also exploit modern comparative statistical methods applied to cultural and behavioural variation [85] and gene-culture coevolutionary theory [22] , [58] , [83] , [86] to reconstruct human evolutionary histories. The function of reliable aspects of human cognition, and of consistent behavioural patterns, can be explored utilizing the same methods. An important point here is that researchers are not restricted to considerations of the current function of evolved traits, and well-established methods are available to reconstruct the evolutionary history of human cognition.
ii) With regard to functional questions, while EP has stressed the idea that human beings are adapted to past worlds [87] , a niche-construction perspective argues that human beings are predicted to build environments to suit their adaptations, and to construct solutions to self-imposed challenges, aided and abetted by the extraordinary level of adaptive plasticity afforded by our capacities for learning and culture [88] . While adaptiveness is far from guaranteed, from this theoretical perspective humans are expected to experience far less adaptive lag than anticipated by EP [88] . If correct, examining the relationship between evolved psychological mechanisms and reproductive success in modern environments will not necessarily be an unproductive task.
Consistent with this hypothesis is the observation that humans have experienced extraordinary levels of population growth, indicative of increments in absolute fitness, in the Holocene whilst exposed to modern, culturally constructed environmental conditions [60] . However, rather than simply pronouncing that human behaviour is, or is not, likely to be adaptive, a modern EP would carry out quantitative analyses across a multitude of behavioural and cognitive traits to measure to what extent, or on what occasions, human behaviour is currently adaptive (e.g., [89] ). We anticipate that the formal methods of human behavioural ecology are likely to be productive even in modern societies, in many instances (e.g., [90] , [91] ). Where the use of optimality models proves unproductive, cultural evolution and gene-culture coevolutionary models could be developed to investigate whether the data conform to equilibria that are not globally optimal (e.g., [92] ). Researchers could go on to explore which factors explain this variation, for instance by measuring, among diverse traits and across a broad range of populations, what percentage of the variance in behaviour is explained by local ecology and what percentage is better predicted by cultural history (e.g., [93] ).
iii) In order to study the causal mechanisms underlying the character, researchers can employ methodologies that are available to modern cognitive psychologists and neuroscientists, such as fMRI and related technology, and take advantage of advances in genetics. While much EP research describes human behaviour in terms of information processing, decision rules and cognition, the psychological adaptations can also be described at the level of the nervous system. Cognitive and behavioural neuroscientists have amassed a huge amount of research on the functioning of the nervous system, including the influence of genes on brain development. However, evolutionary psychologists rarely examine whether their hypotheses regarding evolved psychological mechanisms are supported by what is known about how the brain works. Here the role of evolutionary knowledge is less direct, and again relegated to the generation of novel hypotheses that can be tested using established protocols.
Variation in experimental procedures, patterns of connectivity, differences between individuals, and comparisons across species potentially allows researchers to explore to what extent the circuitry associated with the focal mechanism is human specific, and to identify both the major genes involved and the environmental conditions that regulate their expression. There is evidence that modern neuroscience technologies are starting to be used to test hypotheses generated from evolutionary theory [94] – [97] , and some evolutionary psychologists are beginning to present evolutionary accounts of genetic variation underlying traits such as personality [98] – [100] . The aforementioned developments in cognitive neuroscience and genetics open up further opportunities for a broader EP.
iv) As discussed earlier, development is an extremely important factor in human cognition, and the human mind is built via a constant interplay between the individual and its environment. Recent work by developmental psychologists demonstrates how it is possible to detect the unlearned roots of cognition, such as deep, explicit conceptual understanding, through careful experimentation on young children [44] . Such experiments also reveal the manner in which culturally and individually variable concepts emerge, through domain-general learning akin to bootstrapping, in response to a culturally constructed, symbolically encoded environments [44] . In principle, all posited evolved psychological mechanisms, from fear of snakes to cheater-detection mechanisms, could be subject to the same kind of detailed developmental investigation.
Recent trends in developmental biology and cognitive neuroscience recognize that the human brain and behaviour are shaped to an important extent by individual and social learning [36] . Hitherto, EP's theoretical stance led it to assume domain specificity in cognition, resulting in the neglect of opportunities to investigate to what extent human social and asocial learning are reliant upon processes that apply across domains, or the manner in which cross-domain general learning processes build on domain-specific inputs. For instance, while behavioural innovation is critical to the survival of animals living in changing and unpredictable environments, whether such innovation is channeled in a context specific manner is unclear. Innovation could instead be reliant on domain-general mechanisms expressed in complex cognition, intelligence and learning; for instance, innovation could involve learned behaviour patterns being adapted to a new domain. Available evidence suggests the latter scenario [76] , [101] .
Similarly, EP has engaged in a longstanding debate with advocates of cultural evolution over whether human social learning is governed by evolved content biases (e.g., choose the sugar-rich food) or by domain-general context biases (e.g., conform to the local norm). There is sufficient empirical evidence for the deployment of context biases, such as conformity or prestige bias, to render the casual dismissal of transmitted culture counterproductive [102] , [103] . A broader EP could actively pursue these questions, by testing experimentally whether human social learning is dominated by content or context biases, and by investigating the factors that affect reliance on each. The finding that innovation, social learning, and other aspects of development are capable of introducing novelty into phenotype design space, thereby establishing new selective scenarios [39] , [41] , [48] , opens up new opportunities for investigating evolutionary novelty to which social scientists can actively participate.
Conclusions
None of the aforementioned scientific developments render evolutionary psychology unfeasible; they merely require that EP should change its daily practice. The key concepts of EP have led to a series of widely held assumptions (e.g., that human behaviour is unlikely to be adaptive in modern environments, that cognition is domain-specific, that there is a universal human nature), which with the benefit of hindsight we now know to be questionable. A modern EP would embrace a broader, more open, and multi-disciplinary theoretical framework, drawing on, rather than being isolated from, the full repertoire of knowledge and tools available in adjacent disciplines. Such a field would embrace the challenge of exploring empirically, for instance, to what extent human cognition is domain-general or domain specific, under what circumstances human behaviour is adaptive, how best to explain variation in human behaviour and cognition. The evidence from adjacent disciplines suggests that, if EP can reconsider its basic tenets, it will flourish as a scientific discipline.
- View Article
- Google Scholar
- 2. Cosmides L, Tooby J (1997) Evolutionary psychology: A primer. Available: http://www.psych.ucsb.edu/research/cep/primer.html . Accessed 14 June 2011.
- 3. Laland K. N, Brown G. R (2011) Sense and Nonsense. evolutionary perspectives on human behaviour. Second edition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
- 4. Buller D. J (2005) Adapting minds. Evolutionary psychology and the persistent quest for human nature. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
- 6. Richardson R. C (2007) Evolutionary psychology as maladapted psychology. Cambridge , MA: MIT Press.
- 8. Buller D. J (2009) Four fallacies of pop evolutionary psychology. Scientific American, January. pp. 74–81.
- 9. Rose H, Rose S, editors. (2000) Alas poor Darwin: arguments against evolutionary psychology. London: Cape.
- 10. Fodor J. A (2000) The mind doesn't work that way. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
- 12. Sterelny K (2003) Thought in a hostile world. Malden, MA: Blackwell.
- 13. Gray R. D, Heany M, Fairhall S (2003) Evolutionary psychology and the challenge of adaptive explanation. In: Sterelny K, Fitness J, editors. From mating to mentality. Hove (United Kingdom): Taylor & Francis.
- 14. Endler J (1986) Natural selection in the wild. Monographs in population biology 21. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
- 28. Lewin R, Foley R (2004) Principles of human evolution. Oxford: Blackwell.
- 29. Stringer C, Andrews P (2005) The complete world of human evolution. London: Thames and Hudson.
- 30. Tooby J, Cosmides L (1992) The psychological foundations of culture. In: Barkow J, Cosmides L, Tooby J, editors. The adapted mind: evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. New York: OUP. pp. 19–136.
- 33. Nisbett R. E (2003) The geography of thought. New York: Free Press.
- 37. Edelman G. M (1987) Neural Darwinism: the theory of neuronal group selection. New York: Basic Books.
- 38. Jablonka E, Lamb M (2005) Evolution in four dimensions: genetic, epigenetic, behavioral and symbolic variation in the history of life. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
- 39. Odling-Smee F. J, Laland K. N, Feldman M. W (2003) Niche construction. The neglected process in evolution. Monographs in population biology. 37. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
- 40. Oyama S, Gray R, Griffiths P (2001) Cycles of contingency: developmental systems and evolution. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
- 41. West-Eberhard M. J (2003) Developmental plasticity and evolution. New York: Oxford University Press.
- 44. Carey S (2009) The origin of concepts. New York: Oxford University Press.
- 49. Plotkin H (2002) The imagined world made real: towards a natural science of culture. London: Penguin.
- 54. Plomin R, DeFries J. C, McClearn G. E, McGuffin P (2000) Behavioral Genetics.Fourth edition. New York: Worth Publishers.
- 59. Buss D. M (1994) The evolution of desire. New York: Basic Books.
- 61. Sperber D (1994) Understanding verbal understanding. In: Khalfa J, editor. What is intelligence? Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 179–198.
- 62. Pinker S (1994) The language instinct: how the mind creates language. New York: W. Morrow.
- 63. Fodor J. A (1983) The modularity of mind. An essay on faculty psychology. Cambridge , MA: MIT Press.
- 65. Lefebvre L, Bolhuis J. J (2003) Positive and negative correlates of feeding innovations in birds: evidence for limited modularity. In: Reader S, Laland K. N, editors. Animal innovation. Oxford: Oxford University Press. pp. 39–61.
- 66. Roberts M. J, editor. (2007) Integrating the mind. New York: Psychology Press, Taylor Francis.
- 68. Dickinson A (1980) Contemporary animal learning theory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
- 69. Mackintosh N. J (1974) The psychology of animal learning. New York: Academic Press.
- 70. Rescorla R. A, Wagner A. R (1972) A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In: Black A. H, Prokasy W. F, editors. Classical conditioning II: current research and theory. New York: Appleton. pp. 64–99.
- 71. Striedter G. F (2005) Principles of brain evolution. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer.
- 73. McCorduck P (2004) Machines who think. Second edition. Natick, MA: AK Peters.
- 78. Bolhuis J. J, Verhulst S, editors. (2009) Tinbergen's legacy: function and mechanism in behavioral biology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
- 79. Bolhuis J. J (2009) Function and mechanism in neuroecology: looking for clues. In: Bolhuis , JJ , Verhulst S, editors. Tinbergen's legacy: function and mechanism in behavioral biology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 163–196.
- 81. Laland K. N, Brown G. R (2011) The future of evolutionary psychology. In: Swami V, editor. Evolutionary psychology: a critical introduction. New York: Wiley-Blackwell.
- 83. Richerson P. J, Boyd R (2005) Not by genes alone: how culture transformed human evolution. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
- 84. Burnham K. P, Anderson D (2002) Model selection and multi-model inference. Berlin: Springer Verlag.
- 90. Cronk L, Chagnon N, Irons W, editors. (2000) Adaptation and human behavior: an anthropological perspective. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.
- 92. Boyd R, Richerson P. J (1985) Culture and the evolutionary process. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
- 97. Platek S. M, Keenan J. P, Shackelford T. K, editors. (2007) Evolutionary cognitive neuroscience. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
- 101. Reader S. M, Laland K. N (2003) Animal innovation. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
- 104. Buss D. M (2008) Evolutionary Psychology: the New Science of the Mind. Third edition. London: Allyn and Bacon.
- 105. Tooby J, Cosmides L (2005) Conceptual foundations of evolutionary psychology. In: Buss D. M, editor. The handbook of evolutionary psychology. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. pp. 5–67.
- 112. Lumsden C. J, Wilson E. O (1981) Genes, mind and culture: the coevolutionary process. Cambridge (MA): Harvard University Press.
- 113. Cavalli-Sforza L. L, Feldman M. W (1981) Cultural transmission and evolution. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
- 115. Feldman M. W, Cavalli-Sforza L. L (1989) On the theory of evolution under genetic and cultural transmission with application to the lactose absorption problem. In: Feldman M. W, editor. Mathematical evolutionary theory. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
- 117. Ehrlich P. R (2000) Human natures: Genes, cultures, and the human prospect. Washington, D.C.: Island Press.
- 120. Trivers R. L (1972) Parental investment and sexual selection. In: Campbell B, editor. Sexual selection and the descent of man, 1871–1971. Chicago: Aldine. pp. 136–179.
Theoretical Insights of Evolutionary Psychology: New Opportunities for Studies in Evolutionary Ethnobiology
- Published: 29 January 2020
- Volume 47 , pages 6–17, ( 2020 )
Cite this article
- Joelson Moreno Brito Moura 1 , 4 ,
- Risoneide Henriques da Silva 1 , 4 ,
- Washington Soares Ferreira Júnior 2 ,
- Taline Cristina da Silva 3 &
- Ulysses Paulino Albuquerque ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-8131-8429 4
961 Accesses
7 Citations
1 Altmetric
Explore all metrics
In this article, we present the central ideas of evolutionary psychology, and discuss how their assumptions can help ethnobiologists to understand the dynamic relationship between people and their environments. In this sense, investigating this relationship from an evolutionary perspective can bring new empirical evidence about human evolution, also contributing to both evolutionary psychology and evolutionary ethnobiology.
This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.
Access this article
Subscribe and save.
- Get 10 units per month
- Download Article/Chapter or eBook
- 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
- Cancel anytime
Price includes VAT (Russian Federation)
Instant access to the full article PDF.
Rent this article via DeepDyve
Institutional subscriptions
Similar content being viewed by others
What Do We Study in Evolutionary Ethnobiology? Defining the Theoretical Basis for a Research Program
“Evolutionary Theorizing in Sociology’s Formative Period: Implications for Theorizing Today”
Evolutionary ethnobiology and cultural evolution: opportunities for research and dialog.
Social-ecological systems represent a product of the interaction between sociocultural systems—the set of beliefs, knowledge and behaviors in human groups—and ecological systems—the biotic and abiotic environment of human groups (Berkes and Folke 2000 ).
Cultural evolution is a scientific field that analyzes changes detected in societies from the perspective of Darwinian evolution theory, and that takes into account such aspects as variation, competition, and inheritance (Mesoudi 2011 , 2016 ).
Al-Shawaf, L., Conroy-Beam, D., Asao, K., & Buss, D. M. (2016). Human emotions: An evolutionary psychological perspective. Emotion Review , 8 (2), 1–14.
Google Scholar
Albuquerque, U. P., & Ferreira Júnior, W. S. (2017). What do we study in evolutionary ethnobiology? Defining the theoretical basis for a research program. Evolutionary Biology , 44 (2), 206–215.
Albuquerque, U. P., Medeiros, P. M., & Casas, A. (2015). Evolutionary ethnobiology. In U. P. Albuquerque, P. M. Medeiros, & eA. Casas (Eds.), Evolutionary ethnobiology (pp. 1–5). New York: Springer.
Albuquerque, U. P., Nascimento, A. L. B., Chaves, L. S., Feitosa, I. S., Moura, J. M. B., Gonçalves, P. H. S., et al. (2019). A brief introduction to niche construction theory for ecologists and conservationists. Biological Conservation , 237 , 50–56.
Alqahtani, A. S., Yamazaki, k, Alqahtani, W. H., Tashani, M., Heywood, A. E., Booy, R., et al. (2017). Australian Hajj pilgrims’ perception about mass casualty incidents versus emerging infections at Hajj. Travel Medicine and Infectious Disease , 15 , 81–83.
PubMed Google Scholar
Altman, M. N., Khislavsky, A. L., Coverdale, M. E., & Gilger, J. W. (2016). Adaptive attention: How preference for animacy impacts change detection. Evolution and Human Behavior , 37 , 303–314.
Altman, A., & Mesoudi, A. (2019). Understanding agriculture within the frameworks of cumulative cultural evolution, gene-culture co-evolution, and cultural niche construction. Human Ecology , 47 , 483–497.
Balling, J. D., & Falk, J. H. (1982). Development of visual preference for natural environments. Environment and Behavior , 14 , 5–28.
Barkow, J. H., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (Eds.) (eds.). (1992). The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture . New York: Oxford University Press.
Barrett, H. C. (2012). A hierarchical model of the evolution of human brain specializations. PNAS , 109 (1), 10733–10740.
CAS PubMed Google Scholar
Barrett, H. C., & Broesch, J. (2012). Prepared social learning about dangerous animals in children. Evolution and Human Behavior , 33 , 499–508.
Barrett, H. C., Peterson, C. D., & Frankenhuis, W. E. (2016). Mapping the cultural learnability landscape of danger. Child Development , 87 (3), 770–781.
Berkes, F., & Folke, C. (2000). Linking social and ecological systems: Management practices and social mechanisms for building resilience (p. 476). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Bolhuis, J. J., Brown, G. R., Richardson, R. C., & Laland, K. N. (2011). Darwin in mind: New opportunities for evolutionary psychology. PLoS Biology, 9 , 1–8. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1001109 .
Article CAS Google Scholar
Boyer, P., & Bergstrom, B. (2008). Evolutionary perspectives on religion. Annual Review of Anthropology , 37 (11), 111–130.
Breyer, T. (Ed.) (eds.). (2015). Epistemological dimensions of evolutionary psychology (p. 241). New York: Springer.
Broesch, J., Barrett, H. C., & Henrich, J. (2014). Adaptive content biases in learning about animals across the fife course. Human Nature , 25 (2), 181–199.
Brown, C. (2013). ‘Human nature’, science and international political theory. Journal of International Relations and Development , 16 (4), 435–454.
Buss, D. M. (1989). Sex differences in human mate preferences: Evolutionary hypotheses tested in 37 cultures. Behavioral and Brain Sciences , 12 , 1–49.
Buss, D. M. (1990). Evolutionary social psychology: Prospects and pitfalls. Motivation and Emotion , 14 (4), 265–286.
Buss, D. M. (1995). Evolutionary psychology: A new paradigm for psychological science. Psychological Inquiry , 6 , 1–30.
Buss, D. M., Larsen, R., Westen, D., & Semmelroth, J. (1992). Sex differences in jealousy: Evolution, physiology, and psychology. Psychological Science , 3 , 251–255.
Buss, D. M., & Schmitt, D. P. (1993). Sexual strategies theory: A contextual evolutionary analysis of human mating. Psychological Review , 100 , 204–232.
Bøggild, T., & Laustsen, L. (2016). An intra-group perspective on leader preferences: Different risks of exploitation shape preferences for leader facial dominance. The Leadership Quarterly , 27 , 820–837.
Conroy-Beam, D., & Buss, D. M. (2018). Why is age so important in human mating? Evolved age preferences and their influences on multiple mating behaviors. Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences . https://doi.org/10.1037/ebs0000127 , Advance online publication.
Article Google Scholar
Cosmides, L. (1989). The logic of social exchange: Has natural selection shaped how humans reason? Studies with the Wason selection task. Cognition , 31 , 187–276.
Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1987). From evolution to behavior: Evolutionary psychology as the missing link. In J. Dupre (Ed.), The latest on the best: Essays on evolution and optimaliiy (pp. 277–306). Cambridge: MIT Press.
Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2003). Evolutionary psychology: Theoretical foundations. In L. Nadel (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science (pp. 54–64). London: Macmillan.
DeLecce, T., Barbaro, N., Mohamedally, D., Pham, M. N., & Shackelford, T. K. (2017). Husband’s reaction to his wife’s sexual rejection is predicted by the time she spends with her male friends, but not her male coworkers. Evolutionary Psychology , 15 , 1–5. https://doi.org/10.1177/1474704917705062
Edwards, J. (2003). Evolutionary psychology and politics. Economy and Society , 32 (2), 280–298.
Eisend, M. (2018). Explaining the use and effects of humour in advertising: An evolutionary perspective. International Journal of Advertising , 37 (4), 526–547.
Ferera, M., Baron, A. S., & Diesendruck, G. (2018). Collaborative and competitive motivations uniquely impact infants' racial categorization. Evolution and Human Behavior, 39 (5), 511–519.
Fernandes, N. L., Pandeirada, J. N. S., Soares, S. C., & Nairne, J. S. (2017). Adaptive memory: The mnemonic value of contamination. Evolution and Human Behavior , 38 , 451–460.
Ferreira Júnior, W. S., Medeiros, P. M., & Albuquerque, U. P. (2019). Evolutionary ethnobiology . Chichester: eLS. John Wiley & Sons. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470015902.a0028232
Book Google Scholar
Franek, J. (2016). Methodological consilience of evolutionary ethics and cognitive science of religion. Journal of Cognition and Culture , 16 (1–2), 144–170.
Gangestad, S. W., & Tybur, J. M. (2016). Editorial overview: Evolutionary psychology. Current Opinion in Psychology , 7 , 5–8.
Hagen, E. H., & Bryant, G. A. (2003). Music and dance as a coalition signaling system. Human Nature , 14 (1), 21–51.
Hartmann, P., & Apaolaza-Ibáñes, V. (2010). Beyond savanna: An evolutionary and environmental psychology approach to behavioral effects of nature scenery in green advertising. Journal of Environmental Psychology , 30 (1), 119–128.
Hartmann, P., & Apaolaza-Ibáñes, V. (2013). Desert or rain: Standardization of green advertising versus adaptation to the target audience’s natural environment. European Journal of Marketing , 47 (5/6), 917–933.
Hasford, J., Kidwell, B., & Lopez-Kidwell, V. (2018). Happy wife, happy life: Food choices in romantic relationships. Journal of Consumer Research , 44 (6), 1238–1256.
Hattori, W. T., & Yamamoto, M. E. (2012). Evolution of human behavior: Evolutionary psychology. Estudos de Biologia, Ambiente e Diversidade , 34 (83), 101–112.
Hoffman, E., McCabe, K. A., & Smith, V. L. (1998). Behavioral foundations of reciprocity: Experimental economics and evolutionary psychology. Economic Inquiry , 36 (3), 335–352.
Holbrook, M. B., & O’shaughnessy, J. (1984). The role of emotion in advertising. Psychology and Marketing , 1 , 45–64.
Honing, H., & Ploeger, E. (2012). Cognition and the evolution of music: Pitfalls and prospects. Topics in Cognitive Science , 4 (4), 513–524.
Jeffery, A. J., Shackelford, T. K., Zeigler-Hill, V., Vonk, J., & McDonald, M. (2018). The evolution of human female sexual orientation. Evolutionary Psychological Science , 5 , 1–16. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40806-018-0168-2
King, P. E., Barrett, J. L., Greenway, T. S., Schnitker, S. A., & Furrow, J. L. (2018). Mind the gap: Evolutionary psychological perspectives on human thriving. The Journal of Positive Psychology , 13 (4), 336–345.
Klasios, J. (2016). Evolutionizing human nature. New Ideas in Psychology , 40 , 103–114.
Kubinskia, J. S., Navarrete, C. D., & Jonason, P. K. (2018). Gender differences in two motivational pathways to political conservatism. Personality and Individual Differences , 125 , 145–150.
Laland, K. N., & Brown, G. R. (2006). Niche construction, human behaviour and the adaptive-lag hypothesis. Evolutionary Anthropology, 15 , 95–104.
Laland, K. N., Odling-Smee, J., & Myles, S. (2010). How culture shaped the human genome: Bringing genetics and the human sciences together. Nature Reviews Genetics , 11 (2), 137–148.
Lawrence, P. R., & Pirson, M. (2015). Economistic and humanistic narratives of leadership in the age of globality: Toward a renewed Darwinian theory of leadership. Journal of Business Ethics , 128 (2), 383–394.
Leathers, C. G., & Raines, J. P. (2014). Veblen’s evolutionary economics of religion and the evolutionary psychology of religion. International Journal of Social Economics , 41 (2), 146–161.
Li, N. P., Yong, J. C., Tov, W., Sng, O., Fletcher, G. J., Valentine, K. A., & Balliet, D. (2013). Mate preferences do predict attraction and choices in the early stages of mate selection. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 105 (5), 757.
Li, Y. J., Kenrick, D. T., Griskevicius, V., & Neuberg, S. L. (2012). Economic decision biases and fundamental motivations: How mating and self-protection alter loss aversion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 102 (3), 550.
Mesoudi, A. (2011). Cultural evolution . Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Mesoudi, A. (2016). Cultural evolution: Integrating psychology, evolution and culture. Current Opinion in Psychology , 7 , 17–22.
Moura, J. M. B., Ferreira Júnior, W. S., Silva, T. C., & Albuquerque, U. P. (2017). Landscapes preferences in the human species: Insights for ethnobiology from evolutionary psychology. Ethnobiology and Conservation , 6 (10), 1–7.
Moura, J. M. B., Ferreira Junior, W. S., Silva, T. C., & Albuquerque, U. P. (2018). The influence of the evolutionary past on the mind: An analysis of the preference for landscapes in the human species. Frontiers in Psychology , 9 , 1–13.
Nairne, J. S., & Pandeirada, J. N. S. (2008). Adaptive memory: Is survival processing special? Journal of Memory and Language , 59 (3), 377–385.
Nairne, J. S., Pandeirada, J. N. S., Gregory, K. J., & Vanarsdall, J. E. (2009). Adaptive memory: Fitness relevance and the hunter-gatherer mind. Psychological Science , 20 (6), 740–746.
Nairne, J. S., Pandeirada, J. N., & Thompson, S. R. (2008). Adaptive memory: The comparative value of survival processing. Psychological Science , 19 , 176–180.
Nairne, J. S., Thompson, S. R., & Pandeirada, J. N. S. (2007). Adaptive memory: Survival processing enhances retention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , 33 (2), 263–273.
Nairne, J. S., Vanarsdall, J. E., & Pandeirada, J. N. S. (2012). Adaptive memory: Enhanced location memory after survival processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology , 38 (2), 495–501.
Nouchi, R. (2012). The effect of aging on the memory enhancement of the survival judgment task. Japanese Psychological Research , 54 (2), 210–217.
Orians, G. H., & Heerwagen, J. H. (1992). Evolved responses to landscapes. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 555–579). New York: Oxford University Press.
Petersen, M. B. (2018). Reproductive interests and dimensions of political ideology. Evolution and Human Behavior , 39 (2), 203–211.
Prokop, P., & Fančovičová, J. (2014). Seeing coloured fruits: Utilization of the theory of adaptive memory in teaching botany. Journal of Biological Education , 48 (3), 127–132.
Prokop, P., Fančovičová, J., & Fedor, P. (2014). Parasites enhance self-grooming behaviour and information retention in humans. Behavioural Processes , 107 , 42–46.
Roberts, C. (2015). Psychology, evolution and the traumatised child: Exploring the neurophysiology of early sexual development. Australian Feminist Studies , 30 (86), 377–385.
Roos, L. E., Kim, H. K., Schnabler, S., & Fisher, P. A. (2016). Children’s executive function in a CPS-involved sample: Effects of cumulative adversity and specific types of adversity. Children and Youth Services Review , 71 , 184–190.
PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Ruin, I., Gaillard, J. C., & Lutoff, C. (2007). How to get there? Assessing motorists’ flash flood risk perception on daily itineraries. Environmental Hazards, 7 (3), 235–244.
Saad, G., & Gill, T. (2000). Applications of evolutionary psychology in marketing. Psychology and Marketing , 17 (12), 1005–1034.
Sachs, M. L., Sporrong, S. K., Colding-Jørgensen, M., Frokjaer, S., Helboe, P., Jelic, K., & Kaae, S. (2017). Risk perceptions in diabetic patients who have experienced adverse events: Implications for patient involvement in regulatory decisions. Pharmaceutical Medicine , 31 (4), 245–255.
CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Sandry, J., Trafimow, D., Marks, M. J., & Rice, S. (2013). Adaptive memory: Evaluating alternative forms of fitness-relevant processing in the survival processing paradigm. PLoS ONE , 8 (4), e60868. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0060868
Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Santoro, F. R., Ferreira Júnior, W. S., Ladio, A. H., Araújo, T. A. S., & Albuquerque, U. P. (2015). Does plant species richness guarantee the resilience of local medical systems? A perspective from utilitarian redundancy. PLoS ONE , 10 (3), 1–18.
Santoro, F. R., Santos, G. C., Ferreira-Júnior, W. S., Chaves, L. S., Araújo, T. A. S., Nascimento, A. L. B., et al. (2017). Testing an ethnobiological evolutionary hypothesis on plant-based remedies to treat malaria in Africa. Evolutionary Biology . https://doi.org/10.1007/s11692-016-9400-9
Scheideler, J. K., Taber, J. M., Ferrer, R. A., Grenen, E. G., & Klein, W. M. P. (2017). Heart disease versus cancer: Understanding perceptions of population prevalence and personal risk. Journal of Behavioral Medicine , 40 (5), 839–845.
Schwarz, S., & Hassebrauck, M. (2012). Sex and age differences in mate-selection preferences. Human Nature , 23 (4), 447–466.
Seitz, B. M., Polack, C. W., & Miller, R. R. (2018). Adaptive memory: Is there a reproduction-processing effect? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , 44 (8), 1167–1179.
Silva, R. H., Ferreira Júnior, W. S., Medeiros, P. M., & Albuquerque, U. P. (2019). Adaptive memory and evolution of the human naturalistic mind: Insights from the use of medicinal plants. PLoS ONE , 14 (3), 1–15.
Silva, R. H., Medeiros, P. M., Ferreira Júnior, W. S., & Albuquerque, U. P. (2017). Human mnesic performance in a survival scenario: The application of the adaptive memory concept in ethnobiology. Ethnobiology and Conservation , 9 (6), 1–6.
CAS Google Scholar
Smail, D. L. (2018). The inner demons of the better angels of our nature. Historical Reflections/Réflexions Historiques , 44 (1), 117–127.
Sousa, D. C. P., Soldati, G. T., Monteiro, J. M., Araujo, T. A. S., & Albuquerque, U. P. (2016). Information retrieval during free listing is biased by memory: Evidence from medicinal plants. PLoS ONE , 11 (11), e0165838. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0165838
Stanford, M. (2019). The cultural evolution of human nature. Acta Biotheoretica . https://doi.org/10.1007/s10441-019-09367-7
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Stevenson, R., Oaten, M., Case, T., & Repacholi, B. (2014). Is disgust prepared? A preliminary examination in young children. The Journal of General Psychology , 141 (4), 326–347.
Sugiyama, M. S. (1996). On the origins of narrative. Human Nature , 7 (4), 403–425.
Sütterlin, C., Schiefenhövel, W., Lehmann, C., Forster, J., & Apfelauer, G. (2014). Art as behaviour—an ethological approach to visual and verbal art, music and architecture. Anthropologischer Anzeiger , 71 (1–2), 3–13.
Symons, D. (1987). If we’re all Darwinians, what’s the fuss about. In C. B. Crawford, M. F. Smith, & D. Krebs (Eds.), Sociobiology and psychology (pp. 121–146). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1990). The past explains the present: Emotional adaptations and the structure of ancestral environments. Ethology and Sociobiology , 11 (4–5), 375–424.
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological foundations of culture. In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 19–136). New York: Oxford University Press.
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2005). Conceptual foundations of evolutionary psychology. In D. M. Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 5–67). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2015). The theoretical foundations of evolutionary psychology. In D. M. Buss (Ed.), The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology (pp. 3–87). Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons.
Townsend, J. B., & Barton, S. (2018). The impact of ancient tree form on modern landscape preferences. Urban Forestry & Urban Greening , 34 , 205–216.
Weinstein, J., Bugg, J. M., & Roediger, H. L. (2008). Can the survival recall advantage be explained by basic memory processes? Memory & Cognition , 36 (5), 913–919.
Wilke, A., & Todd, P. M. (2018). Studying the evolution of cognition: Toward more methodological diversity in evolutionary psychology. Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences , 12 (3), 133–134.
Wilson, E. O. (1975). Sociobiology: A new synthesis (p. 720). Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Wixted, J. T., Goldinger, S. D., Squire, L. R., Kuhn, J. R., Papesh, M. H., Smith, K. A., et al. (2018). Coding of episodic memory in the human hippocampus. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America . https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1716443115
Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Yang, L., Lau, K. P. L., & Truong, L. (2014). The survival effect in memory: Does it hold into old age and non-ancestral scenarios? PLoS ONE , 9 (5), 1–9.
Young, S. G., Brown, C. M., & Ambady, N. (2012). Priming a natural or human-made environment directs attention to context-congruent threatening stimuli. Cognition & Emotion, 26 , 927–933.
Download references
Acknowledgements
This study was financed in part by the Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior - Brasil (CAPES) - Finance Code 001. Contribution of the INCT Ethnobiology, Bioprospecting and Nature Conservation, certified by CNPq, with financial support from FACEPE (Foundation for Support to Science and Technology of the State of Pernambuco - Grant Number: APQ-0562-2.01/17). Thanks to CNPq for the productivity grant awarded to UPA. We also acknowledge the CAPES for the grant awarded to JM, and the Fundação de Amparo à Ciência e Tecnologia de Pernambuco (FACEPE) for the grant awarded to RHS. To the anonymous reviewers who improved our paper with their comments.
Author information
Authors and affiliations.
Programa de pós-graduação em Etnobiologia e Conservação da Natureza, Departamento de Biologia, Universidade Federal Rural de Pernambuco, Rua Dom Manoel de Medeiros, s/n, Dois Irmãos, PE, 52171900, Brazil
Joelson Moreno Brito Moura & Risoneide Henriques da Silva
Universidade de Pernambuco, Campus Petrolina, Rodovia BR 203, Km 2, s/n – Vila Eduardo, Petrolina, PE, 56328- 903, Brazil
Washington Soares Ferreira Júnior
Departamento de Biologia, Universidade Estadual de Alagoas, Santana do Ipanema, AL, 57500-000, Brazil
Taline Cristina da Silva
Laboratório de Ecologia e Evolução de Sistemas Socioecológicos (LEA), Departamento de Botânica, Universidade Federal de Pernambuco, Av. Prof. Moraes Rego, 1235, Cidade Universitária, Recife, Pernambuco, 50670-901, Brazil
Joelson Moreno Brito Moura, Risoneide Henriques da Silva & Ulysses Paulino Albuquerque
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
Corresponding authors
Correspondence to Joelson Moreno Brito Moura or Ulysses Paulino Albuquerque .
Additional information
Publisher's note.
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Reprints and permissions
About this article
Moura, J.M.B., da Silva, R.H., Ferreira Júnior, W.S. et al. Theoretical Insights of Evolutionary Psychology: New Opportunities for Studies in Evolutionary Ethnobiology. Evol Biol 47 , 6–17 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11692-020-09491-0
Download citation
Received : 01 July 2019
Accepted : 22 January 2020
Published : 29 January 2020
Issue Date : March 2020
DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/s11692-020-09491-0
Share this article
Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:
Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.
Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative
- Adaptive memory
- Hierarchical memory
- Human evolution
- Naturalist mind
- Social-ecological systems
- Find a journal
- Publish with us
- Track your research
IMAGES
VIDEO
COMMENTS
Scientists at the Center for Evolutionary Psychology specialize in finding new ways that an evolutionary perspective can inform research on the design of the human mind. In so doing, we have been researching many new topics, as well as trying out new approaches to old topics.
This page provides a comprehensive list of evolutionary psychology research paper topics, designed to guide students in exploring the depths of human behavior through the lens of evolution.
Despite its topical diversity, evolutionary psychology has managed to focus on a core group of topics ranging from human social and sexual behavior to (facial) attractiveness and from kinship to altruism.
Evolutionary sport and exercise psychology. Unlike the fields of social psychology or developmental psychology, evolutionary psychology is an overarching theoretical approach to the study of psychology (i.e., a metatheory).
To help answer these questions, we present new empirical research on publication trends in evolutionary psychology’s flagship journal, Evolution and Human Behavior (and its predecessor, Ethology and Sociobiology), from its inception in 1979 to 2008.
Introduction. What do evolutionary psychologists study? Which articles in evolutionary psychology’s flagship journal, Evolution and Human Behavior, have been the most influential? Which variables – if any – are associated with citation counts in evolutionary psychology articles?
Find methods information, sources, references or conduct a literature review on EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY
Evolutionary Psychology is an open access, peer-reviewed journal which focuses on original, empirical research addressing human psychology guided by an evolutionary perspective. View full journal description
We argue that the key tenets of the established EP paradigm require modification in the light of recent findings from a number of disciplines, including human genetics, evolutionary biology, cognitive neuroscience, developmental psychology, and paleoecology.
In this article, we present the central ideas of evolutionary psychology, and discuss how their assumptions can help ethnobiologists to understand the dynamic relationship between people and their environments.