Academia.edu no longer supports Internet Explorer.

To browse Academia.edu and the wider internet faster and more securely, please take a few seconds to  upgrade your browser .

Enter the email address you signed up with and we'll email you a reset link.

  • We're Hiring!
  • Help Center

First page of “Primate archaeology”

Download Free PDF

Primate archaeology

Profile image of Susana Carvalho

2009, Nature

AI-generated Abstract

Primate tool use has profound implications for understanding the evolution of human technology and behavior. This paper introduces 'primate archaeology,' an interdisciplinary field that explores the material culture of all primates to provide a comprehensive framework for studying tool use and its impacts on primate evolution. The comparison between human and non-human primate artifact behaviors unveils new insights into the origins of our species' complex technology and social structures.

Related papers

Since its inception, archaeology has traditionally focused exclusively on humans and our direct ancestors. However, recent years have seen archaeological techniques applied to material evidence left behind by non-human animals. Here, we review advances made by the most prominent field investigating past non-human tool use: primate archaeology. This field combines survey of wild primate activity areas with ethological observations, excavations and analyses that allow the reconstruction of past primate behaviour. Because the order Primates includes humans, new insights into the behavioural evolution of apes and monkeys also can be used to better interrogate the record of early tool use in our own, hominin, lineage. This work has recently doubled the set of primate lineages with an excavated archaeological record, adding Old World macaques and New World capu- chin monkeys to chimpanzees and humans, and it has shown that tool selection and transport, and discrete site formation, are universal among wild stone-tool-using primates. It has also revealed that wild capuchins regularly break stone tools in a way that can make them difficult to distinguish from simple early hominin tools. Ultimately, this research opens up opportunities for the development of a broader animal archaeology, marking the end of archaeology’s anthropocentric era.

The Viking longship is one of the most dominant images of the medieval Scandinavian culture, as evidenced by the various archaeological data, rich iconography and innumerable mentions in contemporary chronicles and Old Norse sagas. However, the majority of archaeological research has focused on the material remains and on the technological aspects of its construction and its practical use. Undoubtedly, the study of the ship itself is a crucial step in the process of understanding the principles of Viking Age shipbuilding and its structural evolution, however, it should be supplemented with an interpretation of the agency those ships had and their influence in society. There is reason to believe that the longship acted as a symbol, not only of social status and wealth but also of authority among Scandinavian rulers of the 9th to the 11th century, triggering a cycle of competition and comparison between them and their vessels. The availability of archaeological evidence of Viking longships provides us with ample information to study the craftsmanship of the era, however, it can only assist in the understanding of Viking Age society to a limited degree. On the other hand, a more in-depth examination of the ship’s abundant imagery and extensive utilization of the written textual evidence can considerably contribute to our investigation regarding the metaphorical and symbolic meaning of the longship as well as its importance to Viking Age society. Consequently, this study concentrates on three aspects: the shipbuilding tradition, the iconography of the ship and the testimonies of the contemporary written sources.

Terceiro Milênio. Revista critica de sociologia e politica., 2018

O presente trabalho propoe uma analise do romance L'Arte delle Gioia, de Goliarda Sapienza, com base em uma perspectiva de critica pos-colonial e feminista em dialogo com o quadro teorico das Epistemologias do Sul (SANTOS, 2014). No seu posicionamento duplamente periferico – no Sul geografico (Meridione) e no Sul Epistemico (mulher) –, o personagem Modesta, e com ela a autora Goliarda, mostra um percurso possivel de reconciliacao com as nossas identidades complexas, de descolonizacao dos nossos destinos.

Der Anschnitt, Beihefte, 2021

Thanks to its extensive ore deposits, the Siegerland has a rich mining and smelting history, whose beginnings date back to the Pre-Roman Iron Age. Large-scale exploitation of ores in the region possibly began as early as the 5th century B.C. and was especially intense from the 3rd to the end of the 1st century B.C., when an economic region focusing on steel production developed. Given the number and state of preservation of sites connected to metallurgy, as well as the degree of specialisation of this mining landscape, the Siegerland can be counted among the most important production areas of Iron Age central Europe. A co-operative project between the Deutsches Bergbau-Museum Bochum, the Ruhr-University Bochum and the LWL-Archäologie für Westfalen, Olpe office, funded by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft, has explored the Iron Age mining landscape of the Siegerland. The project focused on the reconstruction of production chains, from iron ore to the finished product, as well as on the economic area itself and its development. This was achieved through large-scale surveys, geophysical prospection and archaeological excavation of selected sites. In addition, we carried out archaeometallurgical analyses on production residues and the ores themselves, as well as archaeobotanical and geoarchaeological investigations in order to reconstruct the natural landscape. This publication presents the results of the archaeological surveys, excavations and mapping. The work of the project team is considerably broadened through the analysis of prior excavations and maps, beginning with late 19th century work. Most of these investigations could be included and are here published for the first time.

Anglo-Indians are often overlooked in the discourse on the independence of India and Pakistan. However, despite their small numbers, Anglo-Indians punched above their weight and had a significant presence in various critical fields, as well as active and robust and political organisations. This paper traces the development of Anglo-Indian politics in the Punjab, a province which (like Bengal) was partitioned in 1947. Starting with the general Anglo-Indian quest to locate both their identity and nationality in the 1940s, this paper investigates why and how the community leaders in the Punjab not only broke with the central leadership but also argued for a very novel, 'Anglo-Muslim' identity, as they began supporting the Muslim League. This change of stance was much to the chagrin of the central leadership which, by 1947, was firmly tied to the Indian National Congress. This paper thus not only substantially improves our understanding of the politics of the Anglo-Indians during partition, especially at a provincial level, but enables us to understand the manoeuvrings for survival of small communities at a time when the focus seemed to be on only the Muslims and Hindus (and Sikhs in the context of the Punjab).

Proceedings of the ACM/IEEE 42nd International Conference on Software Engineering

Rust, an emerging programming language with explosive growth, provides a robust type system that enables programmers to write memory-safe and data-race free code. To allow access to a machine's hardware and to support low-level performance optimizations, a second language, Unsafe Rust, is embedded in Rust. It contains support for operations that are difficult to statically check, such as C-style pointers for access to arbitrary memory locations and mutable global variables. When a program uses these features, the compiler is unable to statically guarantee the safety properties Rust promotes. In this work, we perform a large-scale empirical study to explore how software developers are using Unsafe Rust in real-world Rust libraries and applications. Our results indicate that software engineers use the keyword unsafe in less than 30% of Rust libraries, but more than half cannot be entirely statically checked by the Rust compiler because of Unsafe Rust hidden somewhere in a library's call chain. We conclude that although the use of the keyword unsafe is limited, the propagation of unsafeness offers a challenge to the claim of Rust as a memory-safe language. Furthermore, we recommend changes to the Rust compiler and to the central Rust repository's interface to help Rust software developers be aware of when their Rust code is unsafe.

Law and Justice , 2018

2018 marks the fiftieth anniversary of the publication of the reissued second edition of Pollock and Maitland's The History of English Law. The first edition was published in 1895 with a second edition in 1898. The 1968 reissue was important because, although the text itself was not revised, a lengthy introduction and select bibliography was added by S F C Milsom. This and subsequent publications by Milsom developed a number of revisions of Maitland's work which Milsom referred to as his 'heresy'. Given the fiftieth anniversary of the reissue and Milsom's death in 2016, it seems timely to reflect upon the legacy of both of these legal historians and the future of the historical study of law in general and the study of the interaction between religion and the law in particular. The objective here is not to provide a detailed appraisal of the lives and work of Maitland and Milsom; for that the reader can look elsewhere. The aim of this reflection is to suggest that the answers for

Religions , 2024

Water intelligence online, 2013

Gerontologia, 2020

JILILE, 2024

IDENTIFICACION DE RIESGOS, 2017

University, 2006

Monthly Weather Review, 1989

Journal of Immunology, 1994

Ciências Humanas e Sociedade: estudos interdisciplinares - Vol. 3, 2024

Therapeutic Advances in Cardiovascular Disease, 2015

Analele Universităţii din Oradea, 2022

Clinical Immunology, 2007

Related topics

  •   We're Hiring!
  •   Help Center
  • Find new research papers in:
  • Health Sciences
  • Earth Sciences
  • Cognitive Science
  • Mathematics
  • Computer Science
  • Academia ©2024

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • View all journals
  • Explore content
  • About the journal
  • Publish with us
  • Sign up for alerts
  • Review Article
  • Published: 16 July 2009

Primate archaeology

  • Michael Haslam 1 ,
  • Adriana Hernandez-Aguilar 1 ,
  • Victoria Ling 1 ,
  • Susana Carvalho 1 ,
  • Ignacio de la Torre 2 ,
  • April DeStefano 3 ,
  • Andrew Du 3 ,
  • Bruce Hardy 4 ,
  • Jack Harris 3 ,
  • Linda Marchant 5 ,
  • Tetsuro Matsuzawa 6 ,
  • William McGrew 1 ,
  • Julio Mercader 7 ,
  • Rafael Mora 8 ,
  • Michael Petraglia 1 ,
  • Hélène Roche 9 ,
  • Elisabetta Visalberghi 10 &
  • Rebecca Warren 4  

Nature volume  460 ,  pages 339–344 ( 2009 ) Cite this article

6963 Accesses

178 Citations

83 Altmetric

Metrics details

All modern humans use tools to overcome limitations of our anatomy and to make difficult tasks easier. However, if tool use is such an advantage, we may ask why it is not evolved to the same degree in other species. To answer this question, we need to bring a long-term perspective to the material record of other members of our own order, the Primates.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Subscribe to this journal

Receive 51 print issues and online access

185,98 € per year

only 3,65 € per issue

Buy this article

  • Purchase on SpringerLink
  • Instant access to full article PDF

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

primate archaeology experiment

Similar content being viewed by others

primate archaeology experiment

Examining the suitability of extant primates as models of hominin stone tool culture

primate archaeology experiment

Reappraising the palaeobiology of Australopithecus

primate archaeology experiment

Functional space analyses reveal the function and evolution of the most bizarre theropod manual unguals

Beck, B. Animal Tool Behavior: The Use and Manufacture of Tools (Garland STPM, 1980)

Google Scholar  

Washburn, S. Tools and human evolution. Sci. Am. 203 , 62–75 (1960)

Article   Google Scholar  

McGrew, W. C. Chimpanzee Material Culture: Implications for Human Evolution (Cambridge Univ. Press, 1992)

Book   Google Scholar  

Kumar, S., Filipski, A., Swarna, V., Walker, A. & Hedges, S. B. Placing confidence limits on the molecular age of the human–chimpanzee divergence. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 102 , 18842–18847 (2005)

Article   CAS   ADS   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Semaw, S. et al. 2.5-million-year-old stone tools from Gona, Ethiopia. Nature 385 , 333–336 (1997)

Article   CAS   ADS   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Delagnes, A. & Roche, H. Late Pliocene hominid knapping skills: the case of Lokalalei 2C, West Turkana, Kenya. J. Hum. Evol. 48 , 435–472 (2005) This paper provides the best evidence so far for the advanced technological skill involved in making some of the world’s oldest known stone artefacts, leading to suggestions that earlier tool manufacture is likely.

Article   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Foley, R. Hominid species and stone-tool assemblages: how are they related? Antiquity 61 , 380–392 (1987)

Panger, M., Brooks, A., Richmond, B. G. & Wood, B. Older than the Oldowan? Rethinking the emergence of hominin tool use. Evol. Anthropol. 11 , 235–245 (2002)

Ottoni, E. & Izar, P. Capuchin monkey tool use: overview and implications. Evol. Anthropol. 17 , 171–178 (2008) This paper is the first study to summarize the burgeoning results from observations of wild-capuchin tool use, placing these into their taxonomic, regional and ecological contexts.

Hohmann, G. & Fruth, B. Culture in bonobos? Between-species and within-species variation in behavior. Curr. Anthropol. 44 , 563–570 (2003)

Teleki, G. Chimpanzee subsistence technology: materials and skills. J. Hum. Evol. 3 , 575–594 (1974)

St Amant, R. & Horton, T. E. Revisiting the definition of animal tool use. Anim. Behav. 75 , 1199–1208 (2008)

Boesch, C. & Boesch, H. Tool use and tool making in wild chimpanzees. Folia Primatol. (Basel) 54 , 86–99 (1990)

Article   CAS   Google Scholar  

McGrew, W. C. The Cultured Chimpanzee: Reflections on Cultural Primatology (Cambridge Univ. Press, 2004)

Hernandez-Aguilar, R. A., Moore, J. & Pickering, T. R. Savanna chimpanzees use tools to harvest the underground storage organs of plants. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 104 , 19210–19213 (2007)

Pruetz, J. D. & Bertolani, P. Savanna chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes verus , hunt with tools. Curr. Biol. 17 , 412–417 (2007)

Article   CAS   PubMed   Google Scholar  

van Schaik, C. P., Fox, E. A. & Sitompul, A. F. Manufacture and use of tools in wild Sumatran orangutans. Implications for human evolution. Naturwissenschaften 83 , 186–188 (1996)

CAS   ADS   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Whiten, A. et al. Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature 399 , 682–685 (1999)

Mannu, M. & Ottoni, E. The enhanced tool-kit of two groups of wild bearded capuchin monkeys in the Caatinga: tool making, associative use, and secondary tools. Am. J. Primatol. 71 , 242–251 (2009)

Breuer, T., Ndoundou-Hockemba, M. & Fishlock, V. First observation of tool use in wild gorillas. PLoS Biol. 3 , e380 (2005)

Article   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Goren-Inbar, N., Werker, E. & Feibel, C. S. The Acheulian Site of Gesher Benot Ya’aqov, Israel: The Wood Assemblage (Oxbow Books, 2002)

Keeley, L. H. & Toth, N. Microwear polishes on early stone tools from Koobi Fora, Kenya. Nature 293 , 464–465 (1981)

Article   ADS   Google Scholar  

Inizan, M.-L., Reduron-Balliner, M., Roche, H. & Tixier, J. Technologie de la Pierre Taillée (CREP, 1995)

Leakey, M. D. Olduvai Gorge. Excavations in Beds I and II, 1960–1963 7 (Cambridge Univ. Press, 1971)

Mora, R. & de la Torre, I. Percussion tools in Olduvai Beds I and II (Tanzania): implications for early human activities. J. Anthropol. Archaeol. 24 , 179–192 (2005)

de Heinzelin, J. et al. Environment and behavior of 2.5-million-year-old Bouri hominids. Science 284 , 625–629 (1999)

Copeland, S. R. Vegetation and plant food reconstruction of lowermost Bed II, Olduvai Gorge, using modern analogs. J. Hum. Evol. 53 , 146–175 (2007)

Backwell, L. R. & d’Errico, F. Evidence of termite foraging by Swartkrans early hominids. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 98 , 1358–1363 (2001)

McGrew, W. C. in Meat-Eating and Human Evolution (eds Stanford, C. & Bunn, H.) 160–178 (Oxford Univ. Press, 2001)

Visalberghi, E. et al. Characteristics of hammer stones and anvils used by wild bearded capuchin monkeys ( Cebus libidinosus ) to crack open palm nuts. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 132 , 426–444 (2007) This paper records physical characteristics of capuchin stone tools, providing the first systematic baseline data for comparison with the better-known chimpanzee and archaeological hominin records.

Boinski, S. et al. in The Biology of Traditions: Models and Evidence (eds Fragaszy, D. & Perry, D.) 365–390 (Cambridge Univ. Press, 2003)

Marchant, L. F. & McGrew, W. C. in Stone Knapping: The Necessary Conditions for a Uniquely Hominin Behaviour (eds Roux, V. & Bril, B.) 341–350 (McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research, 2005) This paper reports on fruit-smashing behaviour by chimpanzees, and outlines a proposal (testable by means of the archaeological record) for the series of steps early hominins may have taken to move from simple anvil use to sophisticated flaked-stone manufacture.

Malaivijitnond, S. et al. Stone-tool usage by Thai long-tailed macaques ( Macaca fascicularis ). Am. J. Primatol. 69 , 227–233 (2007)

de A. Moura, A. & Lee, P. Capuchin stone tool use in caatinga dry forest. Science 306 , 1909 (2004)

Joulian, F. in Modelling the Early Human Mind (eds Mellars, P. & Gibson, K.) 173–189 (McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research, 1996)

Goren-Inbar, N., Sharon, G., Melamed, Y. & Kislev, M. Nuts, nut cracking, and pitted stones at Gesher Benot Ya‘aqov, Israel. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 99 , 2455–2460 (2002)

Wood, B. & Lonergan, N. The hominin fossil record: taxa, grades and clades. J. Anat. 212 , 354–376 (2008)

Leakey, L. S., Tobias, P. V. & Napier, J. R. A new species of the genus Homo from Olduvai Gorge. Nature 202 , 7–9 (1964)

Plummer, T. Flaked stones and old bones: biological and cultural evolution at the dawn of technology. Yearb. Phys. Anthropol. 125 , 118–164 (2004)

Wynn, T. G. & McGrew, W. C. An ape’s view of the Oldowan. Mankind 24 , 383–398 (1989)

Kappelman, J. The evolution of body mass and relative brain size in fossil hominids. J. Hum. Evol. 30 , 243–276 (1996)

Asfaw, B. et al. Australopithecus garhi : a new species of early hominid from Ethiopia. Science 284 , 629–635 (1999)

Wright, R. V. S. Imitative learning of a flaked tool technology: the case of an orangutan. Mankind 8 , 296–306 (1972)

Toth, N., Schick, K. & Semaw, S. in The Oldowan: Case Studies into the Earliest Stone Age (eds Toth, N. & Schick, K.) 155–222 (Stone Age Institute, 2006)

Westergaard, G. C. The stone-tool technology of capuchin monkeys: possible implications for the evolution of symbolic communication in hominids. World Archaeol. 27 , 1–9 (1995)

Schick, K. D. et al. Continuing investigations into the stone tool-making and tool-using capabilities of a bonobo ( Pan paniscus ). J. Archaeol. Sci. 26 , 821–832 (1999)

Tocheri, M. W., Orr, C. M., Jacofsky, M. C. & Marzke, M. W. The evolutionary history of the hominin hand since the last common ancestor of Pan and Homo . J. Anat. 212 , 544–562 (2008)

Nishida, T. The bark-eating habits in primates, with special reference to their status in the diet of wild chimpanzees. Folia Primatol. (Basel) 25 , 277–287 (1976)

Boesch, C. & Boesch, H. Optimization of nut-cracking with natural hammers by wild chimpanzees. Behaviour 83 , 265–286 (1983)

Aiello, L. & Dean, C. An Introduction to Human Evolutionary Anatomy 191 (Academic, 1990)

McBrearty, S. & Jablonski, N. First fossil chimpanzee. Nature 437 , 105–108 (2005)

Schrago, C. G. & Russo, C. A. Timing the origin of New World monkeys. Mol. Biol. Evol. 20 , 1620–1625 (2003)

Mercader, J., Panger, M. & Boesch, C. Excavation of a chimpanzee stone tool site in the African rainforest. Science 296 , 1452–1455 (2002)

Mercader, J. et al. 4,300-year-old chimpanzee sites and the origins of percussive stone technology. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 104 , 3043–3048 (2007) This paper reports the oldest dated chimpanzee activity area, and establishes a series of test criteria for distinguishing human from non-human artefacts that includes the presence of flaking features and distinct use residues.

Carvalho, S., Cunho, E., Sousa, C. & Matsuzawa, T. Chaînes opératoires and resource-exploitation strategies in chimpanzee ( Pan troglodytes ) nut cracking. J. Hum. Evol. 55 , 148–163 (2008) This is so far the most comprehensive study bringing archaeological methodology to the study of primate tools, applying the theoretical concept of operational sequences to chimpanzee nut cracking and highlighting variability in tool function and regional diversity in tool use.

Visalberghi, E. et al. Distribution of potential suitable hammers and transport of hammer tools and nuts by wild capuchin monkeys. Primates 50 , 95–104 (2009)

Liu, Q. et al. Kinematics and energetics of nut-cracking in wild capuchin monkeys ( Cebus libidinosus ) in Piauí, Brazil. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 138 , 210–220 (2009)

Visalberghi, E. et al. Selection of effective stone tools by wild bearded Capuchin monkeys. Curr. Biol. 19 , 213–217 (2009)

Matsuzawa, T. in Chimpanzee Cultures (eds Wrangham, R., McGrew, W. C., de Waal, F. & Heltne, P.) 351–370 (Harvard Univ. Press, 1994)

Blumenschine, R. J. & Selvaggio, M. M. Percussion marks on bone surfaces as a new diagnostic of hominid behaviour. Nature 333 , 763–765 (1988)

Sept, J. M. Was there no place like home? A new perspective on early hominid archaeological sites from the mapping of chimpanzee nests. Curr. Anthropol. 33 , 187–207 (1992)

Sept, J. Shadows on a changing landscape: comparing nesting patterns of hominids and chimpanzees since their last common ancestor. Am. J. Primatol. 46 , 85–101 (1998)

Hernandez-Aguilar, A. Chimpanzee nest distribution and site re-use in a dry habitat: implications for early hominin ranging. J. Hum. Evol. (in the press)

Anderson, J. R. Sleep-related behavioural adaptations in free-ranging anthropoid primates. Sleep Med. Rev. 4 , 355–373 (2000)

McGrew, W. C., McKee, J. & Tutin, C. Primates in caves: two new reports of Papio spp. J. Hum. Evol. 44 , 521–526 (2003)

Whiten, A., Horner, V. & Marshall-Pescini, S. Cultural panthropology. Evol. Anthropol. 12 , 92–105 (2003)

Fragaszy, D., Visalberghi, E. & Fedigan, L. The Complete Capuchin: The Biology of the Genus Cebus (Cambridge Univ. Press, 2004)

Hunt, G. R. Manufacture and use of hook-tools by New Caledonian crows. Nature 379 , 249–251 (1996)

Article   CAS   ADS   Google Scholar  

Mann, J. et al. Why do dolphins carry sponges? PLoS One 3 , e3868 (2008)

Article   ADS   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Rybczynski, N. Woodcutting behavior in beavers (Castoridae, Rodentia): estimating ecological performance in a modern and a fossil taxon. Paleobiology 34 , 389–402 (2008)

Madden, J. R. Do bowerbirds exhibit cultures? Anim. Cogn. 11 , 1–12 (2008)

Hall, K. R. L. & Schaller, G. B. Tool-using behavior of the California sea otter. J. Mamm. 45 , 287–298 (1964)

Hunt, G. R., Sakuma, F. & Shibata, Y. New Caledonian crows drop candle-nuts onto rock from communally used forks on branches. Emu 102 , 283–290 (2002)

Biro, D. et al. Cultural innovation and transmission of tool use in wild chimpanzees: evidence from field experiments. Anim. Cogn. 6 , 213–223 (2003)

Leakey, M. G., Spoor, F. & Brown, F. New hominin genus from eastern Africa shows diverse middle Pliocene lineages. Nature 410 , 433–440 (2001)

Brunet, M. et al. A new hominid from the Upper Miocene of Chad, Central Africa. Nature 418 , 145–151 (2002)

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank R. Foley and the US National Science Foundation Revealing Hominid Origins Initiative. The University of Cambridge, the Galton Institute and King’s College, Cambridge, UK, sponsored the symposium ‘Primatology Meets Palaeoanthropology 2: The Origins of Percussive Technology’, held in October 2008 at the Leverhulme Centre for Human Evolutionary Studies in Cambridge.

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Leverhulme Centre for Human Evolutionary Studies, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 1QH, UK

Michael Haslam, Adriana Hernandez-Aguilar, Victoria Ling, Susana Carvalho, William McGrew & Michael Petraglia

Institute of Archaeology, University College London, London WC1H 0PY, UK

Ignacio de la Torre

Department of Anthropology, Rutgers University, New Brunswick, New Jersey 08901, USA,

April DeStefano, Andrew Du & Jack Harris

Department of Anthropology, Kenyon College, Gambier, Ohio 43022, USA,

Bruce Hardy & Rebecca Warren

Department of Anthropology, Miami University, Oxford, Ohio 45056, USA,

Linda Marchant

Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University, Inuyama, Aichi 484-8506, Japan,

Tetsuro Matsuzawa

Department of Archaeology, University of Calgary, Calgary, Alberta T2N 1N4, Canada,

  • Julio Mercader

Division de Prehistoria, Universidad Autonoma de Barcelona, Barcelona 08193, Spain

Rafael Mora

Maison de l’Archéologie et de l’Ethnologie, CNRS, Paris 92023, France,

Hélène Roche

Istituto di Scienze e Tecnologie della Cognizione, Rome 00197, Italy

Elisabetta Visalberghi

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Michael Haslam .

PowerPoint slides

Powerpoint slide for fig. 1, powerpoint slide for fig. 2, powerpoint slide for fig. 3, powerpoint slide for fig. 4, rights and permissions.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article.

Haslam, M., Hernandez-Aguilar, A., Ling, V. et al. Primate archaeology. Nature 460 , 339–344 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1038/nature08188

Download citation

Issue Date : 16 July 2009

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1038/nature08188

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

This article is cited by

Dialects in leaf-clipping and other leaf-modifying gestures between neighbouring communities of east african chimpanzees.

  • Kirsty E. Graham
  • Catherine Hobaiter

Scientific Reports (2023)

Humans, the Norm-Breakers

  • Kristin Andrews

Biology & Philosophy (2023)

Microbotanical residues for the study of early hominin tools

  • George Belev
  • Andreu Ollé

Scientific Reports (2022)

Modeling a primate technological niche

  • Jonathan S. Reeves
  • Tomos Proffitt
  • Lydia V. Luncz

Scientific Reports (2021)

Sandstone Ground Stone Technology: a Multi-level Use Wear and Residue Approach to Investigate the Function of Pounding and Grinding Tools

  • E. Cristiani
  • A. Zupancich

Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory (2021)

Quick links

  • Explore articles by subject
  • Guide to authors
  • Editorial policies

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

primate archaeology experiment

primate archaeology experiment

Primatology.net

Share this post.

primate archaeology experiment

The Monkey Stone Age: A Glimpse into Primate Archaeology

Documenting primate stone tool use in real-time: the case of coiba national park.

primate archaeology experiment

The white-faced capuchin monkeys of Coiba National Park, Panama, have emerged as unexpected practitioners of stone tool use, offering a unique opportunity for primate archaeology in real-time. This fascinating discovery sheds light on the evolution of tool use among non-human primates and poses intriguing questions about its origins and transmission.

A Botanist's Discovery Leads to New Frontiers in Archaeology

In 2004, Alicia Ibáñez, a botanist exploring Coiba National Park, observed white-faced capuchin monkeys using rocks to crack open sea almonds. This seemingly mundane observation sparked intrigue among scientists, eventually reaching UC Davis anthropologist Nicolas Zwyns and his student Meredith Carlson. Carlson's journey into primate archaeology was born from this serendipitous encounter, leading to groundbreaking research on the origins and spread of stone tool use among capuchins.

primate archaeology experiment

Investigating Percussion Behavior in Capuchin Monkeys

For the past six years, Carlson has delved into the phenomenon of stone tool use among white-faced capuchins inhabiting Coiba and Jicarón islands. While multiple populations exist on each island, only select groups exhibit percussion behavior, utilizing rocks to crack open sea almonds, hermit crabs, and coconuts. This selective adoption raises intriguing questions about the cultural transmission and evolutionary drivers of tool use in primates.

primate archaeology experiment

Carlson's extensive fieldwork involves observing and recording the monkeys' tool use behaviors, meticulously documenting each instance of stone percussion and its outcomes. Through this painstaking process, she aims to understand the underlying motivations and mechanisms driving the adoption and transmission of stone tool use within capuchin communities.

Exploring Origins and Hypotheses

In a paper published by Royal Society Open Science 1 , Zwyns and his team propose four hypotheses regarding the origin and function of stone tool use in animals. These hypotheses range from resource availability to embedded food extraction. Yet, the precise origins of capuchin stone tool use remain elusive, prompting further investigation into the complex interplay of ecological, social, and cognitive factors shaping primate behavior.

primate archaeology experiment

The ongoing research in Coiba National Park offers a valuable opportunity to test and refine these hypotheses, providing valuable insights into the evolutionary pathways that led to the emergence of stone tool use in primates. By analyzing the ecological context, social dynamics, and cognitive abilities of capuchin monkeys, researchers hope to unravel the mysteries of their stone tool traditions and shed light on the broader implications for understanding human evolution.

Primate Archaeology: Bridging Past and Present

Carlson's pioneering work in primate archaeology represents a novel intersection of anthropology and primatology. By documenting stone tool use in real-time and analyzing its archaeological implications, she seeks to unravel the mysteries of human evolution and behavior. Through her fieldwork in Coiba National Park, Carlson offers invaluable insights into the dynamic relationship between living primates and their material culture, illuminating the evolutionary pathways that shaped our ancestors' journey into the Stone Age.

As the field of primate archaeology continues to evolve, Carlson's research stands at the forefront, paving the way for new discoveries and a deeper understanding of the complex behaviors and adaptations that have shaped primate societies throughout history. By bridging the gap between past and present, Carlson and her colleagues are not only unraveling the mysteries of the monkey stone age but also shedding light on the broader dynamics of cultural evolution in both human and non-human primates.

Barrett, B. J., Monteza-Moreno, C. M., Dogandžić, T., Zwyns, N., Ibáñez, A., & Crofoot, M. C. (2018). Habitual stone-tool-aided extractive foraging in white-faced capuchins, Cebus capucinus . Royal Society Open Science , 5 (8), 181002. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.181002

Discussion about this post

primate archaeology experiment

Ready for more?

primate archaeology experiment

IMAGES

  1. Back to the Stone Age: Why study primate archaeology?

    primate archaeology experiment

  2. Discovery of Ancient Kenyan Primate Proconsul Fossils Redefines Ape

    primate archaeology experiment

  3. Primate archaeology: Digging up secrets of the monkey Stone Age

    primate archaeology experiment

  4. Species New to Science: [Mammalogy • 2017] Primate Archaeology Evolves

    primate archaeology experiment

  5. IoA researcher Tomos Proffitt is part of a research team working on

    primate archaeology experiment

  6. Alamogordo Primate Experiment

    primate archaeology experiment

COMMENTS

  1. Primate archaeology

    Primate archaeology is a field of research established in 2008 that combines research interests and foci from primatology and archaeology. ... Since its conception primate archaeology has also implemented the use of captive studies [4] akin to archaeological experiments with non-human primates looking into stone tool manufacture. [5] [6] [7]

  2. A primatological perspective on human cultural origins

    These experiments with great apes were pioneer in the field of experimental primate archaeology as they illustrated the physical abilities of apes for making and using sharp stone tools. However, the experiments described above presented some limitations that complicate the interpretability of their results as well as their implications for ...

  3. Primate archaeology

    Primate archaeology provides the necessary methodology and scope to incorporate these developments into a coherent framework for the systematic location and interpretation of evolutionarily ...

  4. Experimental Primate Archaeology: Detecting

    another set of totally naïve subjects was tutored on key points derived from the first experiment. They scored signifi-cantly higher, showing that monkey artifacts are distinguishable and that discrimination can be easily taught. Non-human as well as human primates have lithic technology, which means that they too have an archaeological record.

  5. (PDF) Primate archeology

    Field experiments exposing wild chimp anzees to a variet y of. ... Primate archaeology places the entirety of human behavioural evolu-tion into its wider comparative biological context, and its ...

  6. Primate archaeology

    Field experiments exposing wild chimpanzees to a variety of raw materials for extractive foraging complement this work59, as does the study of percussion-marked fossil bones60. ... Primate archaeology provides the necessary methodology and scope to incorporate these developments into a coherent framework for the systematic location and ...

  7. Primate archaeology evolves

    Primate archaeology is much more reliant on stone tool evidence than is traditional human archaeology, at least for the past few thousand years, because of human innovations in the use of shell ...

  8. PDF Primate archaeology

    human-primate anatomy, and the reasons why hominins alone have taken tool use to such an extreme. A long-standing separation of anthropocentric archaeology from

  9. PDF Experimental Primate Archaeology: Detecting Stone Handling by Japanese

    EXPERIMENTAL PRIMATE ARCHAEOLOGY: DETECTING ... In the first experiment, human subjects of varying degrees of knowledge of SH were asked to separate handled versus non-handled stones. Overall ...

  10. The Monkey Stone Age: A Glimpse into Primate Archaeology

    As the field of primate archaeology continues to evolve, Carlson's research stands at the forefront, paving the way for new discoveries and a deeper understanding of the complex behaviors and adaptations that have shaped primate societies throughout history. By bridging the gap between past and present, Carlson and her colleagues are not only ...